Journal of Behavioral Medicine

, Volume 20, Issue 1, pp 29–45

The Outward Expression of Anger, the Inward Experience of Anger and CVR: The Role of Vocal Expression

  • Aron Wolfe Siegman
  • Selena Cappell Snow
Article

Abstract

Two hypotheses were tested: (1) that only the outward expression of anger, not its mere experience, is associated with heightened cardiovascular reactivity; and (2) that the discussion of anger-arousing experiences in a mood incongruent speech style (soft and slow) attenuates the subjective experience of anger and its cardiovascular correlates. Each of 24 subjects participated in three experimental conditions: (1) Anger-out, in which previously experienced anger-arousing events were described loudly and quickly; (2) Anger-in, in which anger-arousing events were relived inwardly, in subject's imagination; and (3) mood-incongruent speech, in which anger-arousing events were described softly and slowly. Only the Anger-out condition was associated with high cardiovascular reactivity levels. The Anger-in and the mood-incongruent conditions were associated with near-zero and very low reactivity levels, respectively. Subjective anger ratings were highest in the Anger-out condition, moderate in the Anger-in condition, and lowest (not angry) in the mood-incongruent condition. All differences were significant. These findings suggest that the full-blown expression of anger, in all of its paraverbal intensity, is pathogenic and that the mere inner experience of anger is not.

anger-out anger-in cardiovascular reactivity control of anger 

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

REFERENCES

  1. Borow, K. M., & Newberger, J. W. (1982). Noninvasive estimation of central aortic pressure during the oscillometric method for analyzing systemic artery pulsatory flow: Comparative study of indirect, systolic, diastolic, and mean brachial artery pressure with simultaneous direct ascending aortic pressure measurements. American Heart Journal, 103, 879–886.Google Scholar
  2. Bower, G. H. (1981). Mood and memory. American Psychologist 36, 129–148.Google Scholar
  3. Burns, J. W., & Katkin, E. S. (1993). Psychological, situational, and gender predictors of cardiovascular reactivity to stress: A multivariate approach. Journal of Behavioral Medicine 16: 445–465.Google Scholar
  4. Buss, A. H., & Durkee, A. (1957). An inventory for assessing different kinds of hostility. Journal of Consulting Psychology, 21: 343–349.Google Scholar
  5. Darwin, C. R. (1955). The expression of emotions in man and animals. New York: Philosophical Library (original work published 1896).Google Scholar
  6. Deffenbacher, J. L. (1993). Anger reduction: Issues, assessment and intervention strategies. In A. W. Siegman & T. Smith (Eds.), Anger, hostility, and the heart (pp. 239–269). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
  7. Dembroski, T. M., & MacDougall, J. M. (1983). Behavioral and psychophysiological perspectives on coronary-prone behavior. In T. M. Dembroski, T. H. Schmidt, & G. Blumchen (Eds.), Biobehavioral bases of coronary heart disease (pp. 106–129). New York: Karger.Google Scholar
  8. Dembroski, T. M., MacDougall, J. M., Costa, P. T., & Grandits, G. A. (1989). Components of hostility as predictors of sudden death and myocardial infarction in the Multiple Risk Factor Intervention Trial. Psychosomatic Medicine 51: 514–522.Google Scholar
  9. Edmunds, G., & Kendrick, D. C. (1980). The measurement of human aggressiveness. West Sussex, England: Ellis Horwood.Google Scholar
  10. Ekman, P. (1972). Universals and cultural differences in facial expression of emotion. In J. K. Cole (Ed.), Nebraska Symposium on Motivation (pp. 207–283). Lincoln, NE: University of Nebraska Press.Google Scholar
  11. Feldstein, S., & Welkowitz, J. (1987). A chronography of conversation: In defense of an objective approach. In A. W. Siegman & S. Feldstein (Eds.), Nonverbal behavior and communication (pp. 435–499). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
  12. Helmig, L., Houston, B. K., Vavak, C. R., & Mullin, J. (1991, March). Hostility related variables, self-schemata and CHD. Presented at the meetings of the Society for Behavioral Medicine, Washington, D.C..Google Scholar
  13. Kaplan, J. R., Botchin, M. B., & Manuck, S. B. (1994). Animal models of aggression and cardiovascular disease. In A. W. Siegman & S. Feldstein (Eds.), Nonverbal behavior and communication (pp. 127–148). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
  14. Keppel, G., Saufley, W. H., & Tokunaga, H. (1992). Introduction to design and analysis: A student's handbook (2nd ed.). New York: W. H. Freeman & Co.Google Scholar
  15. Lange, C. G., & James, W. (1962). The emotions. NY: Hafner (original work published in 1922).Google Scholar
  16. Leventhal, H. A. (1986). Perceptual motor theory of emotion. In K. Scherer & P. Ekman (Eds.), Approaches to emotion (pp. 271–292). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
  17. Mendes de Leon, C. F. (1992). Anger and impatience/irritability in patients of low socioeconomic status with acute coronary heart disease. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 15: 273–284.Google Scholar
  18. Musante, L., MacDougall, J. M., Dembroski, T. M., & Costa, P. T., Jr. (1989). Potential for hostility and dimensions of anger. Health Psychology, 8: 343–354.Google Scholar
  19. Plutchik, R. (1986). Emotions: A general psychoevolutionary theory. In K. R. Scherer & P. Ekman (Eds.), Approaches to emotion (pp. 197–220). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
  20. Sarason, I. (1961). Intercorrelations among measures of hostility. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 17: 192–195.Google Scholar
  21. Shachter, J., & Singer, J. (1962). Cognitive, social, and physiological determinants of emotional state. Psychological Review, 69, 379–390.Google Scholar
  22. Siegel, J. M. (1985). The measurement of anger as a multidimensional construct. In M. A. Chesney & R. H. Rosenman (Eds.), Anger and hostility in cardiovascular and behavioral disorders (pp. 59–81). New York: Hemisphere/McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
  23. Siegman, A. W. (1985). Expressive correlates to affective states and traits. In A W. Siegman & S. Feldstein (Eds.), Nonverbal behavior: A multichannel perspective (pp. 37–68). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
  24. Siegman, A. W. (1987a). The pacing of speech in depression. In J. D. Maser (Ed.), Depression and expressive behavior (pp. 83–102). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
  25. Siegman, A. W. (1987b). The telltale voice: Nonverbal messages of verbal communication. In A. W. Siegman & S. Feldstein (Eds.), Nonverbal behavior and communication, 2nd ed. (pp. 351–434). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
  26. Siegman, A. W. (1989). The role of hostility, neuroticism, and speech style in coronary artery disease. In A. W. Siegman & T. M. Dembroski (Eds.), In search of coronary-prone behavior: Beyond Type A. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
  27. Siegman, A. W. (1991). The biological matrix of human communication: Cardiovascular correlates of angry, depressed, anxious, and neutral communications. Symposium (Biological Parameters of Language: Health Contexts) paper read at the IVth International Conference on Social Psychology and Language, University of California, Santa Barbara, Aug. 18–23.Google Scholar
  28. Siegman, A. W. (1992). The role of expressive vocal behavior in negative emotions: Implications for stress management. Paper presented at XXV International Congress of Psychology, Brussels, July 19–24.Google Scholar
  29. Siegman, A. W. (1993a). Cardiovascular consequences of expressing, experiencing, and repressing anger. J. Behav. Med. 16: 539–569.Google Scholar
  30. Siegman, A. W. (I993b). Paraverbal correlates of stress: Implications for stress identification and management. In Goldberger, L., and Breznitz, S. (eds.). Handbook of Stress, Free Press, New York.Google Scholar
  31. Siegman, A. W., and Boyle, S. (1992). The expression of anger and cardiovascular reactivity in men and women: An experimental investigation. Presented at 50th anniversary international meeting of the American Psychosomatic Society, New York.Google Scholar
  32. Siegman, A. W., and Boyle, S. (1993). Voices of fear-anxiety and sadness-depression: The effects of speech-rate and loudness on fear-anxiety and sadness-depression. J. Abnorm. Psychol 102: 430–437.Google Scholar
  33. Siegman, A. W., Feldstein, S., Tommaso, C., Ringel, N., and Lating, J. (1987). Expressive vocal behavior and the severity of coronary artery disease. Psychosom. Med. 49: 545–561.Google Scholar
  34. Siegman, A. W., Dembroski, T. M., and Ringel, N. (1987b). Components of hostility and the severity of coronary artery disease. Psychosom. Med. 49: 127–135.Google Scholar
  35. Siegman, A. W., Dohm, F. A., and Gjesdal, J. (1988, April). The effects of repressive coping style on the relationship between hostility, neuroticism, and cardiovascular reactivity. Symposium paper presented at the annual meetings of the Society for Behavioral Medicine, Boston.Google Scholar
  36. Siegman, A. W., Anderson, R. A., and Berger, T. (1990). The angry voice: Its effects on the experience of anger and cardiovascular reactivity. Psychosom. Med. 52: 631–643.Google Scholar
  37. Siegman, A. W., Townsend, S., and Blumenthal, R. (1996a). Anger-out, Anger-in, hostility, and CHD. Paper presented at the 4th International Congress of Behavioral Medicine, Washington, DC.Google Scholar
  38. Siegman, A. W., Shin, H., and De Grange, C. (1996b). Dimensions of Anger, Cynical Mistrust, and CHD Risk Factors in Young Adults. Unpublished manuscript, University of Maryland Baltimore County.Google Scholar
  39. Spielberger, C. D. (1988). Manual for the State-Trait Anger Expression Inventory (STAXI). Psychological Assessment Resources, Inc. (PAR), Odessa, FL.Google Scholar
  40. Spielberger, C. D., Reheiser, E. C., and Sydeman, S. J. (1995). Measuring the experience, expression and control of anger. In Kassinove, H. (ed), Anger Disorders: Definitions, Diagnosis, and Treatment, Taylor & Francis, Washington, DC.Google Scholar
  41. Teasdale, J. D. (1983). Negative thinking in depression: Cause, effect or reciprocal relationship? Adv. Behav. There. 5: 3–25.Google Scholar
  42. Tomkins, S. S. (1962). Affect, Imagery, Consciousness. I. The Positive Affects, Springer-Verlag, New York.Google Scholar
  43. Williams, R. B. (1989). Biological mechanisms mediating the relationship between behavior and disease. In Siegman, A. W., and Dembroski, T. M. (eds.), In Search of Coronary Prone Behavior, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Hillsdale, NJ.Google Scholar
  44. Williams, R. B. (1994). Basic biological mechanisms. In Siegman, A. W., and Smith, T. M. (eds.), Anger, Hostility, and the Heart, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Hillsdale, NJ, pp. 117–126.Google Scholar
  45. Zajonc, R. B., and McIntosh, D. N. (1992). Some promising questions and some questionable promises. Psychol. Sci. 3: 70–74.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Plenum Publishing Corporation 1997

Authors and Affiliations

  • Aron Wolfe Siegman
    • 1
  • Selena Cappell Snow
    • 1
  1. 1.University of Maryland Baltimore CountyCatonsville

Personalised recommendations