Skip to main content
Log in

Preferences and Children's Use of Gender-Stereotyped Knowledge About Musical Instruments: Making Judgments About Other Children's Preferences

  • Published:
Sex Roles Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

A developmental model of gender-stereotype acquisition (Martin, 1989) proposes that by the age of 8 years children draw upon information about gender-stereotyped interests as well as other children's sex when deciding how much other children would like different activities; younger children rely on sex only when making such decisions. We examined whether the judgments that children made about other children's preferences were different from those that they made about their own preferences for masculine and feminine musical instruments. Three hundred twelve children aged 8–9 years ranked 6 instruments in order of preference, and rated on a 4-point scale how much they would like to play each one. Children were then asked to decide how much other children would like to play each instrument. Only girls' own preferences for feminine instruments differed according to the gender-stereotyping of their most-preferred instrument. Judgments about how much other children would like masculine and feminine instruments did not differ according to those children's gender-stereotyped interest. Children made stereotypical predictions about the preferences of children of unknown sex who played either a masculine or feminine instrument. Implications for a theoretical account of the development of children's gender-stereotypes are discussed.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Abeles, H. F., & Porter, S. Y. (1978). The sex-stereotyping of musical instruments. Journal of Research in Music Education, 26, 65-75.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bauer, P. J., & Coyne, M. J. (1997). When the name says it all: Preschoolers' recognition and use of the gendered nature of common proper names. Social Development, 6, 271-291.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berndt, T. J., & Heller, K. A. (1986). Gender stereotypes and social inferences: A developmental study. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50, 889-898.

    Google Scholar 

  • Biernat, M. (1991). Gender stereotypes and the relationship between masculinity and femininity: A developmental analysis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 61, 351-365.

    Google Scholar 

  • Crowther, R. D., & Durkin, K. (1982). Sex and age related differences in the musical behavior, interests and attitudes towards music of 232 secondary school students. Educational Studies, 8, 131-139.

    Google Scholar 

  • Deaux, K., & Lewis, L. L. (1984). Structure of gender stereotypes: Interrelationships among components and gender label. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 46, 991-1004.

    Google Scholar 

  • Delzell, J. K., & Leppla, D. A. (1992). Gender associations of musical instruments and preferences of fourth-grade students for selected instruments. Journal of Research in Music Education, 40, 93-103.

    Google Scholar 

  • Edelbrock, C., & Sugawara, A. I. (1978). Acquisition of sex-typed preferences in pre-school aged children. Developmental Psychology, 14, 614-623.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gelman, S. A., & Markman, E. M. (1987). Young children's inductions from natural kinds: The role of categories and appearances. Child Development 58, 1532-1541.

    Google Scholar 

  • Griswold, P. A., & Chroback, D. A. (1981). Sex-role associations of music instruments and occupations by gender and major. Journal of Research in Music Education 29, 57-62.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harrison, A. C., & O'Neill, S. A. (2000). Children gender-typed preferences for musical instruments: An intervention study. Psychology of Music, 28, 81-97.

    Google Scholar 

  • Huston, A. C. (1983). Sex-typing. In P. H. Mussen & E. M. Hetherington (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 4. Socialisation, personality, and social behavior (pp. 387-467). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Livesley, W. J., & Bromley, B. D. (1983). Person perception in childhood and adolescence. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lobel, T. E., Bar-David, E., & Gruber, R. (2000). Gender schema and social judgments: A developmental study of children from Hong Kong. Sex Roles, 43, 19-42.

    Google Scholar 

  • Locksley, A., Borgida, E., Brekke, N., & Hepburn, C. (1980). Sex stereotypes and social judgments. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39, 821-831.

    Google Scholar 

  • Martin, C. L. (1989). Children's use of gender-related information in making social judgments. Developmental Psychology, 25, 80-88.

    Google Scholar 

  • Martin, C. L. (1993). New directions for investigating children's gender knowledge. Developmental Review, 13, 184-204.

    Google Scholar 

  • Martin, C. L., Eisenbud, L., & Rose, H. (1995). Children's gender-based reasoning about toys. Child Development, 66, 1453-1471.

    Google Scholar 

  • Martin, C. L., Wood, C. H., & Little, J. K. (1990). The development of gender stereotype components. Child Development, 61, 1891-1904.

    Google Scholar 

  • O'Neill, S. A., & Boulton, M. J. (1996). Boys' and girls' preferences for musical instruments: A function of gender? Psychology of Music, 24, 171-183.

    Google Scholar 

  • Serbin, L. A., Powlishta, K. K., & Gulko, J. (1993). The development of sex typing in middle childhood. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 58(2, Serial No. 232).

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to A. C. Harrison.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Harrison, A.C., O'Neill, S.A. Preferences and Children's Use of Gender-Stereotyped Knowledge About Musical Instruments: Making Judgments About Other Children's Preferences. Sex Roles 49, 389–400 (2003). https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1025168322273

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1025168322273

Navigation