Skip to main content
Log in

Gender and Hedging: From Sex Differences to Situated Practice

  • Published:
Journal of Psycholinguistic Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

In a reanalysis of women's language, Holmes (1995) has argued that women's use of hedges expresses interpersonal warmth and not, as many researchers have maintained, linguistic tentativeness. It is typically men, she suggests, who employ hedges to convey imprecision and incertitude. In this study, we investigated the use of the hedges sort of and you know in a sample of South African students. Holmes's method of analysis was applied to hedging behavior in 52 dyadic conversations. The study consisted of a 2 (Speaker Gender: Male/Female) × 2 (Audience Gender: Male/Female) × 2 (Condition: Competitive/Noncompetitive) between-subjects experimental design. The results showed that contextual influences eclipsed the effects of gender; in fact, no main effects were found for speaker gender. Fewer hedges were deployed in the competitive condition than in the noncompetitive condition. Moreover, perhaps reflecting differences in social status, both sexes used sort of to express tentativeness more frequently when talking to male addressees. When speaking to female addressees, on the other hand, men deployed facilitative you know hedges more readily than women.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

REFERENCES

  • Baumann, M. (1976). Two features of women's speech? In B. L. Dubois & I. Crouch (Eds.), The sociology of the languages of American women (pp. 32–40). San Antonio, TX: Trinity University.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bilous, K. R., & Krauss, R. M. (1988). Dominance and accommodation in the conversational behaviours of same-and mixed-sex dyads. Language and Communication, 8, 183–194.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bodine, A. (1975). Sex differentiation in language. In B. Thorne & N. Henley (Eds.), Language and sex: Difference and dominance (pp. 130–151). Rowley, MA: Newbury House.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boe, S. K. (1987). Language as an expression of caring in women. Anthropological Linguistics, 29, 271–285.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brouwer, D. (1982). The influence of addressee's sex on politeness in language use. Linguistics, 20, 697–711.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brouwer, D., Gerritson, M., & de Haan, D. (1979). Speech differences between women and men: On the wrong track? Language in Society, 8, 33–50.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown, C. E., Dovido, J. F., & Ellyson, S. L. (1990). Reducing sex differences in visual displays of dominance: Knowledge is power. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 16, 358–368.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown, P. (1980). How and why women are more polite: Some evidence from a Mayan community. In S. McConnell-Ginet, R. Borker, & N. Furman (Eds.), Women and language in literature and society (pp. 111–136). New York: Praeger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown, P. (1990). Gender, politeness, and confrontation in Tenejapa. Discourse Processes, 13, 123–141.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown, P., & Levinson, S. (1987). Universals in language use: Politeness phenomena. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cameron, D., McAlinden, F., & O'Leary, K. (1988). Lakoff in context: The social and linguistic function of tag questions. In J. Coates & D. Cameron (Eds.), Women in their speech communities (pp. 74–93). London: Longman Group.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carli, L. L. (1990). Gender, language and influence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59, 941–951.

    Google Scholar 

  • Coates, J. (1986). Women, men and language. London: Longman Group.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, J. (1977). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Crosby, F., & Nyquist, L. (1977). The female register: An empirical study of Lakoff's hypotheses. Language in Society, 6, 313–322.

    Google Scholar 

  • de Klerk, V. (1988). An investigation into the use of expletives by males and females in Grahamstown. South African Journal of Linguistics, 6, 1–18.

    Google Scholar 

  • de Klerk, V. (1991). The myth of the talkative female. South African Journal of Linguistics, 9, 90–95.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dovido, J. F., Brown, C. E., Heltman, K., Ellyson, S. L., & Keating, C. F. (1988). Power displays between men and women in discussions of gender-linked tasks: A multichannel study. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 55, 580–587.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dubois, B. L., & Crouch, I. (1976). The question of tag questions in women's speech: They don't really use more of them do they? Language in Society, 4, 289–294.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fishman, P. M. (1978). Interaction: The work women do. Social Problems, 25, 397–406.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fishman, P. M. (1980). Conversational insecurity. In H. Giles, W. P. Robinson, & P. M. Smith (Eds), Language: Social psychological perspectives (pp. 127–132). Oxford, England: Pergamon Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goffman, E. (1955). On facework: An analysis of ritual elements in social interaction. Reprinted in J. Laver & S. Hutcheson (Eds.), Communication in face to face interaction (pp. 319–346). Harmondsworth, England: Penguin.

  • Günthner, S. (1992). The construction of gendered discourse in Chinese-German interactions. Discourse and Society, 3, 167–191.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hogg, M. A. (1985). Masculine and feminine speech in dyads and in groups: A study of speech style and gender salience. Journal of Language and Social Psychology, 4, 99–112.

    Google Scholar 

  • Holmes, J. (1984a). Women's language: A functional approach. General Linguistics, 24, 149–178.

    Google Scholar 

  • Holmes, J. (1984b). Hedging your bets and sitting on the fence: Some evidence for hedges as support structures. Te Reo, 27, 47–62.

    Google Scholar 

  • Holmes, J. (1986). Functions of you know in women's and men's speech. Language in Society, 15, 1–22.

    Google Scholar 

  • Holmes, J. (1988). Sort of in New Zealand women's and men's speech. Studia Linguistica, 42, 85–121.

    Google Scholar 

  • Holmes, J. (1990). Hedges and boosters in women's and men's speech. Language and Communication, 10, 185–205.

    Google Scholar 

  • Holmes, J. (1995). Women, men and politeness. London: Longman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Keppel, G. (1982). Design and analysis. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kimble, C. E., Yoshikawa, J. C., & Zehr, H. D. (1981). Vocal and verbal assertiveness in same-sex andmixed-sex groups. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 40, 1047–1054.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kottler, A. E. (1990). Contradictory discourses: An analysis of a male/female conversation. In J. Mouton & D. Joubert (Eds.), Knowledge and method in the human sciences (pp. 331–342). Pretoria, South Africa: Human Science Research Council.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kraemer, H. C., & Jacklin, C. N. (1979). Statistical analysis of dyadic behavior. Psychological Bulletin, 86, 217–224.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lakoff, R. (1975). Language and woman's place. New York: Harper & Row.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mabry, E. (1985). The effects of gender composition and task structure on small group interaction. Small Group Behaviour, 16, 75–96.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maltz, D., & Borker, R. (1982). A cultural approach to male-female miscommunication. In J. J. Gumperz (Ed.), Language and social identity (pp. 196–216). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • McMillan, J. R., Clifton, A. K., McGrath, D., & Gale, W. (1977). Women's language: Uncertainty or interpersonal sensitivity and emotionality? Sex Roles, 3, 545–559.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mulac, A., & Lundell, T. L. (1980). Differences in perceptions created by syntactic-semantic productions of male and female speakers. Communication Monographs, 47, 111–118.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mulac, A., Wiemann, J. M., Widenmann, S. J., & Gibson, T. W. (1988). Male/female language differences and effects in same-sex and mixed-sex dyads: The gender-linked language effect. Communication Monographs, 55, 315–335.

    Google Scholar 

  • O'Barr, W. M., & Atkins, B. K. (1980). ‘“Women's language” or “powerless language”’? In S. McConnell-Ginet, R. Borker, & N. Furman (Eds.), Women and language in literature and society (pp. 93–110). New York: Praeger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Philips, S. (1980). Sex differences and language. Annual Review of Anthropology, 9, 523–544.

    Google Scholar 

  • Piliavin, J. A., & Martin, R. R. (1978). The effects of the sex composition of groups on style of social interaction. Sex Roles, 4, 281–296.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pillon, A., Degauquier, C., & Duquesne, F. (1992). Males' and females' conversational behavior in cross-sex dyads: From gender differences to gender similarities. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 21, 147–172.

    Google Scholar 

  • Priesler, B. (1986). Linguistic sex roles in conversation: Social variation in the expression of tentativeness in English. Berlin, Germany: Mouton de Gruyter.

    Google Scholar 

  • Putnam, L. L., & McCallister, L. (1984). Situational effects of task and gender on nonverbal dispay. In Communication Yearbook (Vol. 4, pp. 679–697). New Jersey: International Communication Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rakow, L. F. (1986). Rethinking gender research in communication. Journal of Communication, 36, 11–26.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rosenblum, K. E. (1986) Revelatory or purposive? Making sense of a ‘female register.’ Semiotica, 59, 157–170.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schultz, K., Briere, J., & Sandler, L. (1984). The use and development of sex-typed language. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 8, 327–336.

    Google Scholar 

  • Swann, J. (1988). Talk control: An illustration from the classroom of problems in analyzing male dominance of conversation. In J. Coates & D. Cameron (Eds.), Women in their speech communities (pp. 122–140). London: Longman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tannen, D. (1990). You just don't understand. New York: Ballantine.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thorne, B., & Henley, N. (1975). Difference and dominance: An overview of language, gender and society. In B. Thorne & N. Henley (Eds.), Language and gender: Difference and dominance. Rowley, MA: Newbury House.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thorne, B., Kramarae, C., & Henley, N. (Eds.). (1983). Language, gender and society. Rowley, MA: Newbury House.

    Google Scholar 

  • Uchida, A. (1992). When ‘difference’ is ‘dominance’: A critique of the ‘anti power-based’ cultural approach to sex differences. Language in Society, 21, 547–568.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wiemann, J. M. (1981). Effects of laboratory videotaping procedures on selected conversational behaviors. Human Communication Research, 7, 302–311.

    Google Scholar 

  • Willis, F. N., & Williams, S. T. (1976). Simultaneous talking in conversation and sex of speakers. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 43, 1067–1070.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zimmerman, D., & West, C. (1975). Sex roles, interruptions and silences in conversation. In B. Thorne & N. Henley (Eds.), Language and sex: Difference and dominance (pp. 105–129). Rowley, MA: Newbury House.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Dixon, J.A., Foster, D.H. Gender and Hedging: From Sex Differences to Situated Practice. J Psycholinguist Res 26, 89–107 (1997). https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1025064205478

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1025064205478

Keywords

Navigation