Skip to main content
Log in

Spontaneous Imitation of Fundamental Frequency and Speech Rate by Nonstutterers and Stutterers

  • Published:
Journal of Psycholinguistic Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Speech can be described either in terms of acoustics, as a perceptual outcome, or as a motor event. Central to theories of speech perception and production is an attempt to describe how these aspects of speech are interrelated. The present experiment investigated how the nonstutterers' and stutterers' reproductions of acoustically presented interrogative sentences were influenced by experimental variations of intonation (sentence initial vs. sentence final) and speech rate (normal vs. time compressed). We studied the effects of these stimulus manipulations on the speech rate and fundamental frequency (F0 ) of 10 adult German-speaking nonstutterers and seven stutterers. Experimental manipulations of intonation and speech rate significantly influenced the syllable duration and speech rate of both normal speakers and stutterers. The fundamental frequency of the subjects' responses were also significantly influenced by the intonation of the stimulus. But the stutterers' increase in F0 for stressed syllables was generally less pronounced than that of nonstutterers. These results imply that (a) the subjects not only extract linguistic meaning from intonation but that they also store extragrammatical speech rate information, and (b) the speakers adopt these speech rate variations for their own productions. Generally, these results demonstrate that speech perception is not limited to extracting linguistically invariant information. The results show that speakers actively generate their own prosody and that this generative process is influenced by the prosodic structure of another speaker's antecedent speech. The implications of these results for theories of speech production are discussed.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

REFERENCES

  • Argyle, M. (1969). Social interaction. London, England: Methuen.

    Google Scholar 

  • Batliner, A. (1991). Fokus, Deklination und Wendepunkt. In H. Altmann, A. Batliner, & W. Oppenreider (Eds.), Zur Intonation von Modus und Fokus im Deutschen (pp. 71–86). Tübingen, Germany: Narr Verlag.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bergmann, G. (1986). Studies in stuttering as a prosodic disturbance. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 29, 290–300.

    Google Scholar 

  • Black, J. W. (1949). Loudness of speaking: The effect of heard stimuli on spoken responses. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 39, 311–315.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bock, K. (1986). Syntactic persistence in language production. Cognitive Psychology, 18, 355–387.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bock, K., & Loebell, H. (1990). Framing sentences. Cognition, 35, 1–40.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bosshardt, H. G. (1990). Subvocalization and reading rate differences between stuttering and nonstuttering children and adults. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 33, 776–785.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bosshardt, H. G. (1993). Differences between stutterers' and nonstutterers' short-term recall and recognition performance. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 36, 286–293.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bosshardt, H. G., & Fransen, H. (1996). On-line sentence processing in adults who stutter and adults who do not stutter. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 39, 785–797.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bosshardt, H. G., & Hagen, B. (1986). Einfluss der englischsprachigen Textoberfläche auf ihre deutsche Übersetzung. Archiv für Psychologie, 140, 127–136.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bosshardt, H. G., & Meyer, A. (1985). Experimentelle Untersuchung des Simultandolmetschens: Einheitenbildung und zeitliche Koordination. Archiv für Psychologie, 137, 137–157.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bosshardt, H. G., & Nandyal, I. (1988). Reading rates of stutterers and nonstutterers during silent and oral reading. Journal of Fluency Disorders, 14, 407–420.

    Google Scholar 

  • Buller, D. B., & Aune, R. K. (1988). The effects of vocalics and nonverbal sensitivity on compliance: A speech accommodation theory explanation. Human Communication Research, 14, 301–332.

    Google Scholar 

  • Buller, D. B., & Aune, R. K. (1992). The effects of speech rate similarity on compliance: Application of communication accommodation theory. Western Journal of Communication, 56, 37–53.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cooper, W. E., & Paccia-Cooper, J. M. (1980). Syntax and speech. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fant, G. (1990). The speech code: Segmental and prosodic features. Proceedings of the Conference on Spoken Language Processing, Kobe, Japan (pp. 1389–1397).

  • Feldstein, S. (1972). Temporal patterns of dialogue: Basic research and reconsiderations. In A. W. Siegman & B. Pope (Eds.), Studies in dyadic communication. New York: Pergamon Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ferreira, F. (1993). Creation of prosody during sentence production. Psychological Review, 100, 233–253.

    Google Scholar 

  • Franken, M. C., Boves, L., Peters, H. F. M., & Webster, R. L. (1991). Prosodic features in the speech of post-therapy compared with the speech of nonstutterers. In H. F. M. Peters, W. Houstijn, & C. W. Starkweather (Eds.), Speech motor control and stuttering (pp. 527–534). Amsterdam: Exerpta Medica.

    Google Scholar 

  • Giles, H. (1973). Communicative effectiveness as a function of accented speech. Speech Monographs, 40, 330–331.

    Google Scholar 

  • Giles, H., & Coupland, N. (1991). Language: Contexts and consequences. Milton Keynes: Open University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Giles, H., & Powesland, P. E. (1975). Speech style and social evaluation. London, England: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grimm, H. (1983). Kognitions-und interaktionspychologische Aspekte der Entwicklungsdysphasie. Sprache & Kognition, 3, 169–186.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jäncke, L. (1993). Stottern eine psychologische oder physiologische Störung? In H. S. Johannsen & L. Springer (Eds.), Stottern, Tagungsbericht, Münster 1993 43–59). Ulm, Germany: Phoniartrische Ambulanz.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jäncke, L. (1994). Variability and duration of voice onset time and phonation in stuttering and nonstuttering adults. Journal of Fluency Disorders, 19, 21–38.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jäger, A. O., & Althoff, K. (1983). Wilde-Intelligenz-Test (WIT): Ein Strukturdiagnostikum. Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe, Verlag für Psychologie.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jakobson, R., & Halle, M. (1956). Fundamentals of language. The Hague, The Netherlands, Mouton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kalveram, K. H. (1991). How pathological audio-phoniatric coupling induces stuttering: A model of speech flow control. In H. F. M. Peters, W. Hulstijn, & C. W. Starkweather (Eds.), Speech motor control and stuttering (pp. 163–170). Amsterdam: Exerpta Medica.

    Google Scholar 

  • Karniol, R. (1995). Stuttering, language, and cognition: A review and a model of stuttering as suprasegmental sentence plan aliguement (SPA). Psychological Bulletin, 117, 104–124.

    Google Scholar 

  • Klatt, D. H., & Stefanski, R. A. (1974). How does a mynah bird imitate human speech? Journal of Acoustic Social America, 55, 822–832.

    Google Scholar 

  • Knipschild, M., & Sappok, C. (1991). Akustische Zeichenverarbeitung durch SONA und VERSTEU. Fortschritte der Akustik-DAGA, (pp. 1045–1048). Bad Honnet: DGP-GmbH.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lehrl, S. (1976). Manual zum MWT-B. Erlangen, Germany: Verlag Dr. med. D. Straube.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levelt, W. J. M. (1989). Speaking: From intention to articulation. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levelt, W. J. M., & Kelter, S. (1982). Surface form and memory in question answering. Cognitive Psychology, 14, 78–106.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lieberman, A. M., & Mattingly, I. G. (1985). The motor theory of speech perception revised. Cognition, 21, 1–36.

    Google Scholar 

  • Natale, M. (1975). Convergence of mean vocal intensity in dyadic communications as a function of social desirability. Journal of Personal and Social Psychology, 32, 790–804.

    Google Scholar 

  • Neilson, P. D., Neilson, M. D., & N. J. O'Dwyer (1992). Adaptive model theory: Application to disorders of motor control. In J. J. Summers (Ed.), Approaches to the study of motor control and learning (pp. 495–547). Amsterdam: Elsevier.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perkins, W. H., Kent, R. D., & Curlee, R. F. (1991). A theory of neuropsycholinguistic function in stuttering. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 34, 734–752.

    Google Scholar 

  • Peters, H. F. M., Hulstijn, W., & Starkweather, C. W. (1989). Acoustic and physiological reaction times of stutterers and nonstutterers. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 32, 668–680.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pisoni, D. P. (1990). Effects of talker variability on speech perception: implications for current research and theory. Proceedings of the Conference on Spoken Language Processing, Kobe, Japan (pp. 1399–1407).

  • Postma, A., & Kolk, H. (1993). The covert repair hypothesis: Prearticulatory repair processes in normal and stuttered dysfluencies. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 36, 472–487.

    Google Scholar 

  • Prinz, W. (1990). A common coding approach to perception and action. In O. Neumann & W. Prinz (Eds.), Relationships between perception and action (pp. 167–201). Berlin, Germany: Springer Verlag.

    Google Scholar 

  • Putman, W. B., & Street, R. L., Jr. (1984). The conception and perception of noncontent speech performance: Implications for speech accommodation theory. International Journal of Sociology of Language, 46, 97–114.

    Google Scholar 

  • Starkweather, C. W. (1987). Fluency and stuttering. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Starkweather, C. W. (1995). A simple theory. Journal of Fluency Disorders, 20, 91–116.

    Google Scholar 

  • Street, R. L., Jr. (1982). Evaluation of noncontent speech accommodation. Language & Communication, 2, 13–31.

    Google Scholar 

  • Street, R. L., Jr. (1984). Speech convergence and speech evaluation in fact-finding interviews. Human Communication Research, 11, 139–169.

    Google Scholar 

  • Street, R. L., Jr., & Brady, R. M., (1982). Speech rate acceptance ranges as a function of evaluative domain, listener speech rate and communication context. Communication Monographs, 49, 290–308.

    Google Scholar 

  • Street, R. L., Jr., & Giles, H. (1982). Speech accommodation theory: A social cognitive approach to language and speech behavior. In M. Roloff & C. Berger (Eds.), Social cognition and communication (pp. 193–226). Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Studdert-Kennedy, M. (1986). Development of the speech perceptuomotor system. In B. Lindblom & R. Zetterstrom (Eds.), Precursors of early speech. New York: Stockton Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Webb, J. T. (1972). An investigation of two speech rate measures in an automated standardized interview. In A. W. Siegman & B. Pope (Eds.), Studies in dyadic communication. New York: Pergamon Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wijnen, F. N. K., & Boers, I. (1994). Phonological priming effects in stutterers. Journal of Fluency Disorders, 19, 1–20.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wingate, M. E. (1976). Stuttering: Theory and treatment. New York: Irvington.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wingate, M. (1988). The structure of stuttering. New York: Springer Verlag.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Bosshardt, HG., Sappok, C., Knipschild, M. et al. Spontaneous Imitation of Fundamental Frequency and Speech Rate by Nonstutterers and Stutterers. J Psycholinguist Res 26, 425–448 (1997). https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1025030120016

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1025030120016

Keywords

Navigation