Skip to main content
Log in

A Narwhal Is an Animal: Partial Word Knowledge Biases Adults' Decisions

  • Published:
Journal of Psycholinguistic Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Adults' accuracy on a sentence verification task was measured to assess reliable biases in their responses as a function of their level of knowledge about the target words, as well as adults' ability to reject incorrect ontological category membership for their partially known words. Three levels of word knowledge (known, frontier, and unknown) were assessed in two experiments. Results indicated that participants were conservative decision-makers about sentences using the vocabulary words and that the conservative bias increased in strength as their level of knowledge about the target word increased. When biased guessing was discouraged, results indicated that participants were able to accurately identify both correct and incorrect sentences using targets at all three levels of knowledge, including their partially known words, but were least reliable at rejecting false sentences using unknown targets. Implications for the role of “knowing not” in adults' vocabulary acquisition are discussed.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

REFERENCES

  • Clark, E. (1987). The principle of contrast: A constraint on language acquisition. In B. MacWhinney (Ed.), Mechanisms of language acquisition. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Collins, A. M., & Quillian, M. R. (1969). Retrieval time from semantic memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 8, 240-247.

    Google Scholar 

  • Curtis, M. E. (1987). Vocabulary testing and vocabulary instruction. In M. G. McKeown & M. E. Curtis (Eds.), The nature of vocabulary acquisition (pp. 37-51). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dale, E. (1965). Vocabulary measurement: Techniques and major findings. Elementary English, 42, 395-401.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dolling, J. (1995). Ontological domains, semantic sorts and systematic ambiguity. International Journal of Human-Computer Studies, 43, 785-807.

    Google Scholar 

  • Durso, F. T., & Shore, W. J. (1991). Partial knowledge of word meanings. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 120, 190-202.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fischer, U. (1994). Learning words from contexts and dictionaries: An experimental comparison. Applied Psycholinguistics, 15, 551-574.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grandy, R. E. (1987). In defense of semantic fields. In E. Lepore (Ed.), New directions in semantics (pp. 259-280). Orlando: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grandy, R. E. (1992). Semantic fields, prototypes, and the lexicon. In A. Lehrer & E. F. Kittay (Eds.), Frames, fields, and contrasts: New essays in semantic and lexical organization (pp. 103-122). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Keil, F. C. (1979). Semantic and conceptual development. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kittay, E. F. (1992). Semantic fields and the individuation of content. In A. Lehrer & E. F. Kittay (Eds.), Frames, fields, and contrasts: New essays in semantic and lexical organization (pp. 229-252). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kolers, P. A., & Ostry, D. J. (1974). Time course of loss of information regarding pattern analyzing operations. Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior, 13, 757-759.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kolers, P. A., & Palef, S. R. (1976). Knowing not. Memory & Cognition, 4, 553-558.

    Google Scholar 

  • Landauer, T. K., & Dumais, S. T. (1997). A solution to Plato's problem: The latent semantic analysis theory of acquisition, induction, and representation of knowledge. Psychological Review, 104, 211-240.

    Google Scholar 

  • Loewenthal, K. (1971). A study of imperfectly acquired vocabulary. British Journal of Psychology, 62, 225-233.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lund, K., & Burgess, C. (1996). Producing high-dimensional semantic spaces from lexical co-occurrence. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 28, 203-208.

    Google Scholar 

  • McNicol, D. (1972). A primer of signal detection theory. London: Allen and Unwin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nagy, W. E., Herman, P. A., & Anderson, R. C. (1985). Learning words from context. Reading Research Quarterly, 20, 233-253.

    Google Scholar 

  • O'Brien, R. G., & Kaiser, M. K. (1985). MANOVA method for analyzing repeated measures designs: An extensive primer. Psychological Bulletin, 97, 313-333.

    Google Scholar 

  • O'Connor, J. (1940). Unsolved business problems. Boston, MA: Johnson O' Connor Research Foundation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sachs, H. J. (1943). The reading method of acquiring vocabulary. Journal of Educational Research, 36, 457-464.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shore, W. J., & Durso, F. T. (1990). Partial knowledge in vocabulary acquisition: General constraints and specific detail. Journal of Educational Psychology, 82, 315-318.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shore, W. J., & Kempe, V. (1999). The role of sentence context in accessing partial knowledge of word meanings. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 28, 145-163.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sines, L. K. (1958). Intelligence tests correlates of Shipley-Hartford performance. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 14, 399-404.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith, P. H., Whitmore, J., Shore, W. J., Robinson, C., & Dixon, W. E. Jr. (1999). Infants' responses to objects representing levels of word knowledge and categorical information. Infant Behavior and Development, 22, 511-526.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sternberg, R. J. (1987). Most vocabulary is learned from context. In M. G. Mckeown & M. E. Curtis (Eds.), The nature of vocabulary acquisition (pp. 89-105). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Trembly, D. (1966). Laws of learning general and specialized vocabularies. In Proceedings of the Meeting of the American Psychological Association (pp. 229-230). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • Whitmore, J. M., & Shore, W. J. (November, 1997). The ontological nature of partial knowledge of word meanings. Presented at the annual meeting of the Psychonomic Society, Philadelphia.

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Lockett, J.N., Shore, W.J. A Narwhal Is an Animal: Partial Word Knowledge Biases Adults' Decisions. J Psycholinguist Res 32, 477–496 (2003). https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1024855830894

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1024855830894

Navigation