Skip to main content

Are Women Perceived as Engaging in More Maladaptive Worry Than Men? A Status Interpretation

We’re sorry, something doesn't seem to be working properly.

Please try refreshing the page. If that doesn't work, please contact support so we can address the problem.

Abstract

Studies 1 and 2 addressed people's perceptions of women's and men's overall maladaptive worry. In Study 1, participants rated how they perceive women's and men's worry in general. In Study 2, participants rated the worry of either a woman or a man whom they know well. As expected in both studies, participants perceived women as experiencing more maladaptive worry than men. A second objective was to compare people's perceptions of women's and men's worry to their perceptions of low- and high-status individuals' worry to determine if a status model can account for the perception that women worry more than men. In Study 3, participants were presented a minimal instantiation of status (Conway, Pizzamiglio, & Mount, 1996). As expected, participants perceived low-status individuals as experiencing more maladaptive worry than high-status individuals. Findings are discussed in terms of gender stereotypes and theoretical models of gender.

This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution.

REFERENCES

  • Averill, J. R. (1982). Anger and aggression: An essay on emotion. New York: Springer Verlag.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berger, J., Cohen, B. P.,& Zelditch, M. (1972). Status characteristics and social interaction. American Sociological Review, 37, 241-255.

    Google Scholar 

  • Borkovec, T. D., Robinson, E., Pruzinsky, T., & Depree, J. A. (1983). Preliminary exploration of worry: Some characteristics and processes. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 21, 9-16.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brody, L. R., & Hall, J. A. (2000). Gender and emotion. In M. Lewis & J. M. Haviland-Jones (Eds.), Handbook of emotions(2nd ed., pp. 338-349). New York: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Broverman, I. K., Broverman, D. M., Clarkson, R. E., Rosenkrantz, P. S., & Vogel, S. R. (1970). Sex-role stereotypes and clinical judgements of mental health. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 34, 1-7.

    Google Scholar 

  • Conway, M., DiFazio, R., & Mayman, S. (1999). Judging others' emotions as a function of the others' status. Social Psychology Quarterly, 62, 291-305.

    Google Scholar 

  • Conway, M., Pizzamiglio, M. T., & Mount, L. (1996). Status, communality, and agency: Implications for stereotypes of gender and other groups. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 25-38.

    Google Scholar 

  • Conway, M., & Vartanian, L. R. (2000). A status account of gender stereotypes: Beyond communality and agency. Sex Roles, 43, 181-199.

    Google Scholar 

  • Davey, G. C. L. (1993). A comparison of three worry questionnaires. Behavior and Research Therapy, 31, 51-56.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eagly, A. H. (1987). Sex differences in social behavior: A social-role interpretation. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eagly, A. H., Wood, W., & Diekman, A. B. (2000). Social role theory of sex differences and similarities: A current appraisal. In T. Eckes & H. M. Trautner (Eds.), The developmental social psychology of gender (pp. 123-174). London: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Geis, F. L. (1993). Self-fulfilling prophecies: A social psychological view of gender. In A. E. Beall & R. J. Trautner (Eds.), The psychology of gender (pp. 9-54). New York: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Glick, P., & Fiske, S. T. (1999). Sexism and other „isms”: Independence, status, and the ambivalent content of stereotypes. In W. B. Swann, J. H. Langlois, & L. A. Gilbert (Eds.), Sexism and stereotypes in modern society: The gender science of Janet Taylor Spence (pp. 193-222). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jacobs, J. A. (1999). The sex segregation of occupations: Prospects for the 21st century. In G. N. Powell (Ed.), Handbook of gender and work (pp. 125-141). London: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kessler, S. J., & McKenna, W. (1978). Gender: An ethnomethodological approach. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lenney, E. (1991). Sex roles: The measurement of masculinity, femininity, and androgyny. In J. P. Robinson, P. R. Shaver, & L. S. Wrightsman (Eds.), Measures of personality and social psychological attitudes (pp. 573-660). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Merriam-Webster's collegiate dictionary (10th ed.). (1993). Springfield, MA: Merriam-Webster.

  • Meyer, T. J., Miller, M. L., Metzger, R. L., & Borkovec, T. D. (1990). Development and validation of the Penn State Worry Questionnaire. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 28, 487-495.

    Google Scholar 

  • Molina, S., & Borkovec, T. D. (1994). The Penn State Worry Questionnaire: Psychometric properties and associated characteristics. In G. C. L. Davey & F. Tallis (Eds.), Worrying: Perspectives and theory, assessment, and treatment (pp. 265-283). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Öhman, A. (2000). Fear and anxiety: Evolutionary, cognitive, and clinical perspectives. In M. Lewis & J. M. Haviland-Jones (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (2nd ed., pp. 573-593). New York: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rhoodie, E. M. (1989). Discrimination against women: A global survey. Jefferson, NC: McFarland.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ridgeway, C. J., & Diekema, D. (1992). Are gender differences status differences? In C. J. Ridgeway (Ed.), Gender, interaction, and inequality (pp. 157-180). New York: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Robichaud, M., & Dugas, M. J. (2000, November). Do men and women worry differently? An investigation of gender effects on a cognitive-behavioral model of worry and anxiety. Poster presented at the annual meeting of the Association for the Advancement of Behavior Therapy, New Orleans, LA.

  • Segerstrom, S. C., Tsao, J. C. I., Alden, L. E., & Craske, M. G. (2000). Worry and rumination: Repetitive thought as a concomitant and predictor of negative mood. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 24, 671-688.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shaver, P., Schwartz, J., Kirson, D., & O'Connor, C. (1987). Emotion knowledge: Further exploration of a prototype approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 1061-1086.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spence, J. T., Helmreich, R. L., & Holahan, C. K. (1979). Negative and positive components of psychological masculinity and femininity and their relationships to self-reports of neurotic and acting out behaviors. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 1673-1682.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stavosky, J. M., & Borkovec, T. G. (1987). The phenomenon of worry: Theory, research and treatment, and its implications for women. Women and Therapy, 6, 77-95.

    Google Scholar 

  • Strauss, J., Muday, T., McNall, K., & Wong, M. (1997). Response style theory revisited: Gender differences and stereotypes in rumination and distraction. Sex Roles, 36, 771-792.

    Google Scholar 

  • Valian, V. (1998). Why so slow? The advancement of women. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wagner, D. G., & Berger, J. (1997). Gender and interpersonal task behaviours: Status expectation accounts. Sociological Perspectives, 40, 1-32.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zelditch, M. (1992). Interpersonal power. In E. Borgatta & M. Borgatta (Eds.), Encyclopedia of sociology (Vol. 2, pp. 994-1001). New York: Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Michael Conway.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and Permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Conway, M., Wood, WJ., Dugas, M. et al. Are Women Perceived as Engaging in More Maladaptive Worry Than Men? A Status Interpretation. Sex Roles 49, 1–10 (2003). https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1023901417591

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1023901417591

  • gender differences
  • status
  • worry