Skip to main content
Log in

Recall Readiness in Children with Autism

  • Published:
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

When people are asked to learn information they need to judge when they have encoded the information accurately and will be able to retrieve it correctly. Making such a judgment is an aspect of metacognitive ability, and is referred to as “recall readiness.” Previous researchers have not considered recall readiness in children with autism, therefore we asked matched groups of children with autism, children with mental retardation, and normally developing children (mean mental age: 7 years) to study several pictures of objects until they felt ready to recall all the objects without error. Their recall was then tested. The children with autism and the children with mental retardation had impaired recall readiness compared to the normally developing children. We discuss this result with reference to other research into the metacognitive abilities of children with autism.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

REFERENCES

  • Baron-Cohen, S. (1989), Are autistic children “behaviourists”? An examination of their mental-physical and appearance-reality distinctions. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 19, 579–600.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bennetto, L., Pennington, B. F., & Rogers, S. J. (1996). Intact and impaired memory functions in autism. Child Development, 67, 1816–1835.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown, A. L., & Barclay, C. R. (1976). The effects of training specific mnemonics on the metamnemonic efficiency of retarded children. Child Development, 47, 71–80.

    Google Scholar 

  • Campione, J. C. (1987). Metacognitive components of instructional research with problem learners. In F. E. Weinert & R. H. Kluwe (Eds.), Metacognition, motivation and understanding (pp. 117–140). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dunn, L. M., Whetton, C., & Pintilie, D. (1982). British Picture Vocabulary Scale. London: NFER-Nelson.

    Google Scholar 

  • Farrant, A., Boucher, J., & Blades, M. (1999). Metamemory in children with autism. Child Development, 70, 107–131.

    Google Scholar 

  • Flavell, J. H., Friedrichs, A. G., & Hoyt, J. D. (1970). Developmental changes in memorization processes. Cognitive Psychology, 1, 324–340.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jarrold, C., Boucher, J., & Russell, J. (1997). Language profiles in children with autism: Theoretical and methodological implications. Autism, 1, 57–76.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kreutzer, M. A., Leonard, C., & Flavell, J. H. (1975). An interview study of children's knowledge about memory. Monographs of the Society for the Research in Child Development, 40(1, Serial No. 159).

  • Markman, E. M. (1973) Factors affecting the young child's ability to monitor his memory. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Pennsylvania.

  • Minshew, N. J., & Goldstein, G. (1993). Is autism an amnesic disorder? Evidence from the California Verbal Learning Test. Neuropsychology, 7, 209–216.

    Google Scholar 

  • Minshew, N. J., Goldstein, G., Muenz, L. R., & Payton, J. B. (1992). Neuropsychological functioning in nonmentally retarded autistic individuals. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 14, 749–761.

    Google Scholar 

  • Minshew, N., Goldstein, G., Siegel, D. J., & Nicholson, M. (1994). Selective pattern of cognitive functioning in autism: a function of information processing load. Biological Psychiatry, 35, 632.

    Google Scholar 

  • Murphy, M. D., Sanders, R. E., Gabriesheski, A. S., & Schmitt, F. A. (1981). Metamemory in the aged. Journal of Gerontology, 36, 185–193.

    Google Scholar 

  • Murphy, M. D., Schmitt, F. A., Caruso, M. J., & Sanders, R. E. (1987). Metamemory in older adults: The role of monitoring in serial recall. Psychology and Aging, 4, 331–339.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perner, J. (1991). Understanding the representational mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pennington, B. F., & Ozonoff, S. (1996). Executive functions and developmental psychopathology. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 37, 51–87.

    Google Scholar 

  • Russell, J. (1996). Agency. Its role in mental development. Hove, Sussex, UK: Taylor and Francis

    Google Scholar 

  • Russell, J., Jarrold, C., & Henry, L. (1996). Working memory in children with autism and with moderate learning difficulties. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 37, 673–686.

    Google Scholar 

  • Snodgrass, J. G., & Vanderwart, M. (1980). A standardized set of 260 pictures: Norms for name agreement, image agreement, familiarity and visual complexity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 6, 174–215.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wellman, H. M. (1985). The child's theory of mind: The development of conceptions of cognition. In S. R. Yussen (Ed.), The growth of reflection in children (pp. 169–206). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Mark Blades.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Farrant, A., Blades, M. & Boucher, J. Recall Readiness in Children with Autism. J Autism Dev Disord 29, 359–366 (1999). https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1023074726548

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1023074726548

Navigation