Skip to main content
Log in

Interference of Sentence and Picture Contexts on Learning Prepositions

  • Published:
Journal of Behavioral Education Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The effects of extra stimulus cues, such as pictures and sentences, on learning of preposition words were examined in two experiments. A one-way repeated-measures design was applied in the study. The results show that both sentences and pictures interfered with and blocked the learning of these context-dependent words in the simultaneous presentations. The superiority of feedback presentations over simultaneous presentations indicate that feedback techniques neutralize the blocking effect. More interestingly, however, the prepositions were learned more quickly in the absence of any context cues and this suggested that it is better to teach basic preposition words either by the presentation of them alone or if extra stimulus cues are to be used they should be presented as feedback.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

REFERENCES

  • Arlin, M., Scott, M., & Webster, J. (1978–1979). The effects of pictures on rate of learning sight words: A critique of the focal attention hypothesis. Reading Research Quarterly, 14, 645–660.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baddeley, A. D. (1986). Working Memory. Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baddeley, A. D. (1992). Working Memory. Science, 255, 556–559.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beimiller, A. (1994). Some observations on beginning reading instruction. Educational Psychologist, 29, 203–209.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bieger, G. R., & Glock, M. D. (1986). Comprehending spatial and contextual information in picture-text instructions. Journal of Experimental Education, 54, 181–188.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bond, G. L., & Tinker, M. (1973). Reading Difficulties: Their Diagnosis and Correction. New York: NY: Appleton Century Crofts.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bransford, J. D., & Johnson, M. K. (1972). Contextual prerequisites for understanding: Some investigations of comprehension and recall. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 11, 711–726.

    Google Scholar 

  • Browder, D. M., & Lalli, J. S. (1991). Review of research on sight word instruction. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 12, 203–228.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ceprano, M. A. (1981). Context versus isolation methods of word instruction: Efficiency assessed by paralleling assessment modes. Journal of Reading Behavior, 13, 381–390.

    Google Scholar 

  • Concannon, S. J. (1975). Illustrations in books for children: Review of research. The Reading Teacher, 29, 254–256.

    Google Scholar 

  • Crowder, R. G., & Morton, J. (1969). Precategorical acoustic storage (PAS). Perception and Psychophysics, 5, 365–373.

    Google Scholar 

  • Deno, S. L. (1968). Effects of words and pictures as stimuli in learning language equivalents. Journal of Educational Psychology, 59, 202–206.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Donald, D. R. (1979). Effects of illustrations on early oral reading accuracy, strategies and comprehension. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 49, 282–289.

    Google Scholar 

  • Donald, D. R. (1983). The use and value of illustrations as contextual information for readers at different progress and developmental levels. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 53, 175–185.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Durkin, D. (1972). Teaching Young Children to Read. Boston, MA: Allyn and Bacon.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ehri, L. C. (1978). Beginning reading from a psycholinguistic perspective: Amalgamation of word identities. In F. B. Murray (Eds.), The Development of the Reading Process (pp. 1–33). Newark, DE: International Reading Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ehri, L. C., & Saltmarsh, J. (1995). Beginning readers outperform older disabled readers in learning to read words by sight. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 7, 295–326.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goodman, K. S. (1965). A linguistic study of miscues in reading. Elementary English, 42, 639–643.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gormezano, I., & Kehoe, E. J. (1981). Classical conditioning and the law of contiguity. In P. Harzem & M. D. Zeiler (Eds.), Advances in Analysis of Behavior. Vol. 2, Predictability, Correlation, and Contiguity (pp. 1–45). Chichester: John Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gough, P. B. (1984). Word recognition. In P.D. Pearson, R. Barr, M. Kamil, & P. Mosenthal (Eds.) Handbook of Reading Research (pp. 225–253). New York: Longman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harris, A. J., & Sipay, E. R. (1975). How to Increase Reading Ability. New York, NY: David Mckay Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hartley, R. N. (1970). Effects of list types and cues on the learning of word lists. Reading Research Quarterly, 6, 97–121.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harzem, P., Lee, I., & Miles, T. R. (1976). The effects of pictures on learning to read. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 46, 318–322.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hayes, D. A., & Readance, J. E. (1983). Transfer of learning from illustration-dependent text. Journal of Educational Research, 76, 245–248.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jespersen, O. (1964). Language: Its Nature, Development and Origin (pp. 138, 211). George Allen & Unwin LTD, London.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kamin, L. J. (1968). “Attention-like” processes in classical conditioning. In M. R. Jones (Eds.), Miami Symposium on the Prediction of Behavior: Aversive Stimulation (pp. 9–31). Miami: University of Miami Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kamin, L. J. (1969). Predictability, surprise, attention and conditioning. In B.A. Campbell & R. M. Church (Eds.), Punishment and Aversive Behavior (pp. 279–296). New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kehoe, E.J. (1987). “Selective association” in compound stimulus conditioning with the rabbit. In I. Gormezano, W. F. Prokasy & R. F. Thompson (Eds.), Classical Conditioning (3rd edition), (pp. 161–196). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kibby, M. W. (1989). Teaching sight vocabulary with and without context before silent reading: A field test of the “focal of attention” hypothesis. Journal of Reading Behavior, 21, 261–278.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lang, R. J., & Solman, R. T. (1979). Effect of pictures on learning to read common nouns. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 49, 138–149.

    Google Scholar 

  • Laughery, K. R., & Pinkus, A. L. (1966). Short-term memory: Effects of acoustic similarity, presentation rate and presentation mode. Psychonomic Science, 6, 285–286.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lesgold, A. M., DeGood, H., & Levin, J. R. (1977). Pictures and young children's prose learning: A supplementary report. Journal of Reading Behavior, 9, 353–360.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lesgold, A. M., Levin, J. R., Shimron, J., & Guttmann, J. (1975). Pictures and young children's learning from oral prose. Journal of Educational Psychology, 67, 636–642.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mackintosh, N. J. (1975). A theory of attention: Variations in the associability of stimuli with reinforcement. Psychological Review, 82, 276–298.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller, W. A. (1938a). Reading with and without pictures. Elementary School Journal, 38, 676–682.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller, W. A. (1938b). What children see in pictures. Elementary School Journal, 39, 280–288.

    Google Scholar 

  • Montare, A., Elman, E., & Cohen, J. (1977). Words and pictures: A test of Samuels' findings. Journal of Reading Behavior, 9, 269–285.

    Google Scholar 

  • Murdock, B. B., Jr. (1967). Auditory and visual stores in short-term memory. Acta Psychologica, 27, 316–324.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Murdock, B. B., Jr., & Walker, K. D. (1969). Modality effects in free recall. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 8, 665–676.

    Google Scholar 

  • Negin, G. A. (1981). Students' ability to recognize words in isolation and in context. Reading Improvement, 18, 73–80.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nemko, B. (1984). Context versus isolation: Another look at beginning readers. Reading Research Quarterly, 19, 461–467.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nicholson, T. (1986). Reading is not a guessing game: The great debate revisited. Reading Psychology, 7, 197–210.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nicholson, T. (1991). Do children read words better in context or in lists? A Classic study revisited. Journal of Educational Psychology, 83, 444–450.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nicholson, T., Bailey, J., & McArthur, J. (1991). Context cues in reading: The gap between research and popular opinion. Journal of Reading, Writing and Learning Disabilities, 7, 33–41.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nicholson, T., & Hill, D. (1985). Good readers don't guess: Taking another look at the issue of whether children read words better in context or in isolation. Reading Psychology, 6, 181–198.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nicholson, T., Lillas, C., Rzoska, A. (1988). Have we been misled by miscues? The Reading Teacher, 42, 6–10.

    Google Scholar 

  • O'Keefe, E. J., & Solman, R. T. (1987). The influence of illustrations on children's comprehension of written stories. Journal of Reading Behavior, 19, 353–377.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pearson, P. D. (1978). On bridging gaps and spanning chasms. Curriculum Inquiry, 8, 353–362.

    Google Scholar 

  • Peeck, J. (1974). Retention of pictorial and verbal content of a text with illustrations. Journal of Educational Psychology, 66, 880–888.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Penney, C. G. (1975). Modality effects in short-term verbal memory. Psychological Bulletin, 82, 68–84.

    Google Scholar 

  • Penney, C. G. (1980). Order of report in bisensory verbal short-term memory. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 34, 190–195.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Penney, C. G. (1989a). Modality effects and the structure of short-term verbal memory. Memory & Cognition, 17(4), 398–422.

    Google Scholar 

  • Penney, C. G. (1993). Interactions between presentation modality and encoding task in frequency judgments. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 46A(3), 517–532.

    Google Scholar 

  • Penney, C. G., & Butt, A. K. (1986). Within-and between-modality associations in probed recall: A test of the separate-streams hypothesis. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 40(1), 1–11.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Purnell, K. N., & Solman, R. T. (1991). The influence of technical illustrations on students; comprehension in geography. Reading Research Quarterly, 26, 277–299.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rayner, K., & Pollatsek, A. (1989). The Psychology of Reading. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reid, D. J., Briggs, N., & Beveridge, M. (1983). The effect of picture on the readability of a school science topic. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 53, 327–335.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reinking, D. R., Hayes, D. A., & McEneaney, J. E. (1988). Good and poor readers' use of explicit and graphic aids. Journal of Reading Behavior, 20, 229–243.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rescorla, R. A., & Wagner, A. R. (1972). A theory of Pavlovian conditioning: Variations in the effectiveness of reinforcement and nonreinforcement. In A. H. Black, & W. F. Prokasy (Eds.), Classical Conditioning II: Current Research and Theory (pp. 64–99). New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rudy, J. W. (1974). Stimulus selection in animal conditioning and P-A learning: variations in the associative process. Journal of Verbal learning and Verbal Behavior, 13, 282–296.

    Google Scholar 

  • Samuels, S. J. (1967). Attentional processes in reading: The effects of pictures on the acquisition of reading responses. Journal of Educational Psychology, 58, 337–342.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Samuels, S. J. (1970). Effects of pictures on learning to read, comprehension and attitudes. Review of Educational Research, 40, 397–407.

    Google Scholar 

  • Saunders, R. J., & Solman, R. T. (1984). The effect of pictures on the acquisition of a small vocabulary of similar sight-words. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 54, 265–275.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shankweiler, D., & Liberman, I. Y. (1972). Misreading: A search for causes. In J. F. Kavanagh, & I. G. Mattingly (Eds.), Language by Ear and by Eye: The Relationships between Speech and Reading, (pp. 293–317). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Singer, H. (1980). Sight word learning with and without pictures: A critique of Arlin, Scott and Webster's research. Reading Research Quarterly, 15, 290–298.

    Google Scholar 

  • Singer, H., Samuels, S. J., & Spiroff, J. (1973–1974). The effect of pictures and contextual conditions on learning responses to printed words. Reading Research Quarterly, 9, 555–567.

    Google Scholar 

  • Singh, N. N., & Solman, R. T. (1990). A stimulus control analysis of the picture-word problem in children who are mentally retarded: The blocking effect. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 23, 525–532.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Solman, R. T., & Adepoju, A. A. (1995). The effect of aural feedback in second language learning. The Journal of Behavioral Education, 5, 433–445.

    Google Scholar 

  • Solman, R. T., & Chung, K. K. H. (1996). Language transfer and blocking in second language vocabulary learning. The Journal of Behavioral Education, 6, 173–190.

    Google Scholar 

  • Solman, R. T., Singh, N. N., & Kehoe, E. J. (1992). Pictures block the learning of sightwords. Educational Psychology, 12, 143–153.

    Google Scholar 

  • Solman, R. T., & Wu, H. M. (1995). Pictures as feedback in single word learning. Educational Psychology, 15, 227–244.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sperling, G. (1963). A model for visual memory tasks. Human Factors, 5, 19–31.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sperling, G. (1967). Successive approximation to a model for short-term memory. Acta Psychologica, 27, 285–292.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vernon, M. D. (1953). The value of pictorial illustration. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 23, 180–187.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vernon, M. D. (1954). The instruction of children by pictorial illustration. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 24, 171–179.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weintraub, S. (1960). The effect of pictures on the comprehension of second-grade basal reader. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Illinois, Urbana-Champaign.

  • Willows, D. M., Borwick, D., & Hayvren, M. (1981). The contents of readers. In T. G. Waller & G. E. Mackinnon (Eds.): Reading Research Advances in Theory and Practice (Vol. 2), (pp. 97–175). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, R. M., & Hall, M. A. (1972). Reading and the Elementary School Child, Theory and Practice for Teachers. New York, NY: Van Nostrand Reinhold Co.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wu, H. M., & Solman, R. T. (1993). Effective use of pictures as extra stimulus prompts. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 63, 144–160.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Zhang, Q., Elliott, R.T. Interference of Sentence and Picture Contexts on Learning Prepositions. Journal of Behavioral Education 8, 439–456 (1998). https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1022805316027

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1022805316027

Navigation