A Negative Mood Induction Procedure with Efficacy Across Repeated Administrations in Women

  • Suzanna Hernandez
  • Jillon S. Vander Wal
  • Bonnie Spring

Abstract

Although several experimental techniques are effective in inducing negative mood, none has established efficacy over repeated testing. The lack of mood induction tools validated across repeated administrations impedes study of emotional changes in the same individual over time. A brief, sad mood induction combining music and an autobiographical memory was administered to 17 women on 6 occasions (two 3-day sets over a 2-week period of time). Mood was measured before and after the induction procedure. Repeated measures ANOVAs showed that the induction produced a large increase in POMS depression (d = 1.48) that recurred consistently across all testing days. A brief negative mood induction combining music and autobiographical memory appears suitable for use in studies that involve repeated administrations.

mood induction negative affect depression repeated measures 

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

REFERENCES

  1. Boyle, G. J. (1987). A cross-validation of the factor structure of the Profile of Mood States: Were the factors correctly identified in the first instance? Psychological Reports, 60(2), 343-354.Google Scholar
  2. Brewer, D., Doughtie, E. B., & Lubin, B. (1980). Induction of mood and mood shift. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 36, 215-226.Google Scholar
  3. Buchwald, A. M., Strack, J., & Coyne, J. C. (1981). Demand characteristics and the Velten mood induction procedure. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 49, 478-479.Google Scholar
  4. Cash, T. F., Rimm, D. C., & MacKinnon, R. (1986). Rational–irrational beliefs and the effects of the Velten mood induction procedure. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 10, 461-467.Google Scholar
  5. Clark, D. M. (1983). On the induction of depressed mood in the laboratory: Evaluation and comparison of the Velten and musical procedures. Advances in Behavioral Research and Therapy, 5, 27-49.Google Scholar
  6. Cohen, J. (1977). Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  7. Eich, E., Macaulay, D., & Ryan, L. (1994). Mood dependent memory for events of the personal past. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 123, 201-215.Google Scholar
  8. Ellenbogen, M. A., Young, S. N., Dean, P., Palmour, R. M., & Benekelfat, C. (1996). Mood response to acute tryptophan depletion in health volunteers: Sex differences and temporal stability. Neuropsychopharmacology, 15(5), 465-474.Google Scholar
  9. First, M. B., Spitzer, R. L., Gibbon, M., & Williams, J. B. W. (1995). Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis I Disorders–-Patient Edition (SCID-I/P, Version 2.0). New York: New York State Psychiatric Institute, Biometrics Division.Google Scholar
  10. Gerrards-Hesse, A., Kordelia, S., & Hesse, F. W. (1994). Experimental inductions of emotional states and their effectiveness: A review. British Journal of Psychology, 85, 55-78.Google Scholar
  11. Gilboa-Schechtman, E., Revelle, W., & Gotlib, I.H. (2000). Stroop interference following mood induction: Emotionality, mood congruence and concern relevance. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 24, 491-502.Google Scholar
  12. Gomez, R., Cooper, A., & Gomez, A. (2000). Susceptibility to positive and negative mood states: Test of Eysenck's, Gray's and Newman's theories. Personality and Individual Differences, 29, 351-366.Google Scholar
  13. Ingram, R. E., & Ritter, J. (2000). Vulnerability to depression: Cognitive reactivity and parental bonding in high-risk individuals. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 109, 588-596.Google Scholar
  14. Kassel, J. D., & Unrod, M. (2000). Smoking, anxiety, and attention: Support for the role of nicotine in attentionally mediated anxiolysis. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 109(1), 161-166.Google Scholar
  15. Kenealy, P. M. (1986). The Velten mood induction procedure: A methodological review. Motivation and Emotion, 10, 315-335.Google Scholar
  16. Larsen, R. J., & Sinnett, L. M. (1991). Meta-analysis of experimental manipulations: Some factors affecting the Velten mood induction procedure. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 17, 323-334.Google Scholar
  17. Martin, M. (1990). On the induction of mood. Clinical Psychology Review, 10, 669-697.Google Scholar
  18. McNair, D. M., Lorr, M., & Droppleman, L. F. (1971). The Manual for the Profile of Mood States (POMS). San Diego, CA: Educational and Industrial Testing Services.Google Scholar
  19. Nolen-Hoeksema, S., & Morrow, J. (1993). Effects of rumination and distraction on naturally occurring depressed mood. Cognition and Emotion 7(6), 561-570.Google Scholar
  20. Norcross, J. C., Guadagnoli, E., & Prochaska, J. O. (1984). Factor structure of the Profile of Mood States (POMS): Two partial replications. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 40(5), 1270-1277.Google Scholar
  21. Simes, R. J. (1986). An improved Bonferroni procedure for multiple tests of significance. Biometrica, 73(3), 751-754.Google Scholar
  22. Slyker, J. P., & McNally, R. J. (1991). Experimental induction of anxious and depressed moods: Are Velten and musical procedures necessary? Cognitive Therapy and Research, 15(1), 33-45.Google Scholar
  23. Spitzer, R. L., Williams, J. B., Gibbon, M., & First, M. B. (1994). The Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV (SCID-IV). New York: New York State Psychiatric Institute, Biometrics Division.Google Scholar
  24. Westermann, R., Spies, K., Stahl, G., Hesse, F. W. (1996). Relative effectiveness and validity of mood induction procedures: A meta-analysis. European Journal of Social Psychology, 26(4), 557-580.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Plenum Publishing Corporation 2003

Authors and Affiliations

  • Suzanna Hernandez
    • 1
  • Jillon S. Vander Wal
    • 1
  • Bonnie Spring
    • 1
  1. 1.University of Illinois at ChicagoChicago

Personalised recommendations