Skip to main content
Log in

The Nature of Regularity and Irregularity: Evidence from Hebrew Nominal Inflection

  • Published:
Journal of Psycholinguistic Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Most evidence for the role of regular inflection as a default operation comes from languages that confound the morphological properties of regular and irregular forms with their phonological characteristics. For instance, regular plurals tend to faithfully preserve the base's phonology (e.g., rat-rats), whereas irregular nouns tend to alter it (e.g., mouse-mice). The distinction between regular and irregular inflection may thus be an epiphenomenon of phonological faithfulness. In Hebrew noun inflection, however, morphological regularity and phonological faithfulness can be distinguished: Nouns whose stems change in the plural may take either a regular or an irregular suffix, and nouns whose stems are preserved in the plural may take either a regular or an irregular suffix. We use this dissociation to examine two hallmarks of default inflection: its lack of dependence on analogies from similar regular nouns, and its application to nonroots such as names. We show that these hallmarks of regularity may be found whether or not the plural form preserves the stem faithfully: People apply the regular suffix to novel nouns that do not resemble existing nouns and to names that sound like irregular nouns, regardless of whether the stem is ordinarily preserved in the plural of that family of nouns. Moreover, when they pluralize names (e.g., the Barak-Barakim), they do not apply the stem changes that are found in their homophonous nouns (e.g., barak-brakim “lightning”), replicating an effect found in English and German. These findings show that the distinction between regular and irregular phenomena cannot be reduced to differences in the kinds of phonological changes associated with those phenomena in English. Instead, regularity and irregularity must be distinguished in terms of the kinds of mental computations that effect them: symbolic operations versus memorized idiosyncrasies. A corollary is that complex words are not generally dichotomizable as “regular” or “irregular” different aspects of a word may be regular or irregular depending on whether they violate the rule for that aspect and hence must be stored in memory.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Subscribe and save

Springer+ Basic
$34.99 /Month
  • Get 10 units per month
  • Download Article/Chapter or eBook
  • 1 Unit = 1 Article or 1 Chapter
  • Cancel anytime
Subscribe now

Buy Now

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

REFERENCES

  • Alegre, M., & Gordon, P. (1996). Red rats eater exposes recursion in children's word formation. Cognition, 60, 65–82.

    Google Scholar 

  • Aronoff, M. (1994). Morphology by itself. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bat-El, O. (1994). Stem modification and cluster transfer in modern Hebrew. Natural Language & Linguistic Theory, 12, 571–596.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bat-El, O. (1996). Selecting the best of the worst: The grammar of Hebrew blends. Phonology, 13, 283–328.

    Google Scholar 

  • Benua, L. (1997). Transderivational identity: Phonological relations between words. Doctoral dissertation, University of Massachusetts, Amherst. (Available as ROA-235 on http://ruccs.rutgers.edu/roa.html)

    Google Scholar 

  • Berent, I., Pinker, S., & Shimron, J. (1999). Default nominal inflection in Hebrew: Evidence for mental variables. Cognition, 72, 1–44.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bolozky, S. (1978). Some aspects of modern Hebrew phonology. In R. A. Berman (Ed.), Modern Hebrew Structure (11–67). Tel Aviv: Universities Publishing Projects.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bybee, J. (1985). Morphology: A study of the relation between meaning and form. Philadelphia: Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bybee, J., & Moder, C. (1983). Morphological classes as natural categories. Language, 59, 251–270.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, N., & Halle, M. (1968). The sound pattern of English. New York: Harper and Row.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clahsen, H., & Almazen, M. (1998). Syntax and morphology in Williams syndrome. Cognition, 68, 167-198.

    Google Scholar 

  • Daugherty, K., & Seidenberg, M. (1992). Rules or connections? The past tense revisited.Proceedings of the 14th Annual Conference of Cognitive Science Society, Hillsdale, NJ.

  • Gordon, P. (1985). Level ordering in lexical development. Cognition, 21, 73–93.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gordon, P., & Alegre, M. (1999). Is there a dual system for regular inflections? Brain and Languages, 68(1), 212–218, </del>doi: 10.1006/brln.1999.2082.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goshen, M., Livne, Z., & Shafan, S. (1970). Hadikduk ha'Ivri hashimushi. (Hebrew grammar). Jerusalem: Shoken.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hare, M., Elman, J., & Daugherty, K. (1995). Default generalization in connectionist networks. Language & Cognitive Processes, 10, 601–630.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kempley, S. T., & Morton, J. (1982). The effects of priming with regular and irregular related words in auditory word recognition. British Journal of Psychology, 73, 441–454.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kim, J., Marcus, G., Pinker, S., Hollander, M., & Coppola, M. (1994). Sensitivity of children's inflection to grammatical structure. Journal of Child Language, 21, 179–209.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kim, J., Pinker, S., Prince, A., & Prasada, S. (1991). Why no mere mortal has ever flown out to center field. Cognitive Science, 15, 173–218.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lieber, R. (1980). On the organization of the lexicon. Doctoral dissertation, Department of Linguistics and Philosophy, MIT, Cambridge. Distributed by the Indiana University Linguistics Club.

    Google Scholar 

  • MacWhinney, B., & Leinbach, J. (1991). Implementations are not conceptualizations: Revising the verb learning model. Cognition, 40, 121–171.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marchman, V. (1997). Children's productivity in the English past tense: The role of frequency, phonology, and neighborhood structure. Cognitive Science, 21(3), 283–305.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marcus, G. (1998). Rethinking eliminative connectionism. Cognitive Psychology, 37, 243–282, doi: 10.1006/cogp.1998.0694.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marcus, G. (2001). The algebraic mind: Integrating connectionism and cognitive science. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marcus, G., Brinkmann, U., Clahsen, H., Wiese, R., & Pinker, S. (1995). German inflection: The exception that proves the rule. Cognitive Psychology, 29, 189–256, doi: 10.1006/cogp.1995.1015.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marcus, G. F., Pinker, S., Ullman, M., Hollander, M., Rosen, T. J., & Xu, F. (1992). Overregularization in language acquisition. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 57(4), 181.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marslen-Wilson, W. D., & Tyler, L. K. (1997). Dissociating types of mental computation. Nature, 387, 592–594.

    Google Scholar 

  • McCarthy, J., & Prince, A. (1990). Foot and word in prosodic morphology: The Arabic broken plural. Natural Language & Linguistic Theory, 8, 209–283.

    Google Scholar 

  • McCarthy, J., & Prince, A. (1995a). Prosodic morphology. In J. A. Goldsmith (Ed.), Phonological theory (318–366). Oxford: Basil Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • McCarthy, J., & Prince, A. (1995b). Faithfulness and reduplicative identity. ROA-60, http://ruccs.rutgers.edu/roa.html

  • Orsolini, M., & Marslen-Wilson, W. (1997). Universals in morphological representation: Evidence from Italian. Language & Cognitive Processes, 12(1), 1–48.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pastizzo, M. J., & Feldman, L. B. (2002). Discrepancies between orthographic and unrelated baselines in masked priming undermine a decompositional account of morphological facilitation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 28, 244–249.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pinker, S. (1991). Rules of language. Science, 253, 530–535.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pinker, S. (1999). Words and rules: The ingredients of language. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pinker, S., & Prince, A. (1988). On language and connectionism: Analysis of parallel distributed processing model of language acquisition. Cognition, 28, 73–193.

    Google Scholar 

  • Plunkett, K., & Juola, P. (1999). A connectionist model of English past tense and plural morphology. Cognitive Science, 23(4), 463–490.

    Google Scholar 

  • Plunkett, K., & Marchman, V. (1991). U-shaped learning and frequency effects in a multilayered perceptron: Implications for child language acquisition. Cognition, 38, 43–102.

    Google Scholar 

  • Plunkett, K., & Marchman, V. (1993). From rote learning to system building. Cognition, 48, 21–69.

    Google Scholar 

  • Plunkett, K., & Nakisa, C. (1997). A connectionist model of Arabic plural system. Language & Cognitive Processes, 12, 807–836.

    Google Scholar 

  • Prasada, S., & Pinker, S. (1993). Generalization of regular and irregular morphological patters. Language & Cognitive Processes, 8, 1–56.

    Google Scholar 

  • Prince, A., & Smolensky, P. (1997). Optimality: From neural networks to Universal Grammar. Science, 275, 1604–1610.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rueckl, J., Mikolinski, M., Raveh, M., Miner, C., & Mars, F. (1997). Morphological priming, fragment completion, and connectionist networks. Journal of Memory & Language, 36, 382–405, doi: 10.1006/jmla.1996.2489.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rumelhart, D. E., & McClelland, J. L. (1986). On learning past tense of English verbs: Implicit rules or parallel distributed processing? In J. L. McClelland, D. E. Rumelhart, & T. P. R. Group (Eds.), Parallel distributed processing: Explorations in the microstructure of cognition (Vol. 2 Psychological and biological models, 216–271). Cambridge MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Senghas, A., Kim, J. J., Pinker, S., & Collins, C. (1991). Plurals-inside-compounds: Morphological constraints and their implications for acquisition. Paper presented at the Sixteenth Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development, October 18-20, Boston, MA.

  • Sonnenstuhl, I., Eisenbeiss, S., & , H. (1999). Morphological priming in the German mental lexicon. Cognition, 72(3), 203–237.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stanners, R. F., Neiser, J. J., Hernon, W. P., & Hall, R. (1979). Memory representation for morphologically related words. Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior, 18, 399–412.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stemberger, J. P. (1995). Phonological and lexical constraints on morphological processing. In L. B. Feldman (Ed.), Morphological aspects of language processing (247–267). Hillsdale, NJ.

  • Stemberger, J. P. (1998). Morphology in language production with special reference to connectionism. In A. Spencer and A. M. Zwicky (Eds.), The Handbook of Morphology (428–452). Oxford: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ullman, M. (1999). Acceptability ratings for regular and irregular past tense forms: Evidence for a dual-system model of language from word frequency and phonological neighborhood effects. Language & Cognitive Processes, 14, 47–67.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ullman, M., Bergida, R., & O'Craven, K. M. (1997). Distinct fMRI activation for regular and irregular past tense. NeuroImage, 5, S549.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ullman, M., T., & Gopnik, M. (1999). Inflectional morphology in a family with inherited specific language impairment. Applied Psycholinguistics, 20(1), 51–117.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ussishkin, A. (in press). The inadequacy of the consonantal root: Modern Hebrew denominal verbs and output-output correspondence. Phonology, 16(3).

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Iris Berent.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Berent, I., Pinker, S. & Shimron, J. The Nature of Regularity and Irregularity: Evidence from Hebrew Nominal Inflection. J Psycholinguist Res 31, 459–502 (2002). https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1021256819323

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1021256819323

Navigation