Skip to main content
Log in

Effects of Game Type on Children's Gender-Based Peer Preferences: A Naturalistic Observational Study

  • Published:
Sex Roles Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Children's propensity for interacting with peersof their own gender is a robust phenomenon, evidentacross many contexts. This study investigated howchildren's gender-based peer preferences varied as a function of a contextual variable —type of children's game. Using naturalisticobservations, 242 first- to third-graders (identicalnumbers of boys and girls, mostly White andmiddle-class) were observed playing two games that varied in theirphysicality and competitiveness; games were structuredto have equal numbers of boys and girls available asplaymates. As predicted, boys and girls interactedsignificantly more with same-sex than other-sex peers. Ourprediction of a game-type effect was partiallysupported: in the more competitive and physical game,boys chose same-sex playmates significantly more oftenthan in the less competitive/physical game, butgirls' same-sex peer preference did not vary across thegames. The findings provide further support for thenecessity of investigating social context as aninfluence on children's own-sex favoritism.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

REFERENCES

  • Buhrmester, D., & Furman, W. (1987). The development of companionship and intimacy. Child Development, 58, 1101-1113.

    Google Scholar 

  • Charlesworth, W. R., & Dzur, C. (1987). Gender comparisons of preschoolers' behavior and resource utilization in group problem-solving. Child Development, 58, 191-200.

    Google Scholar 

  • Colley, A., Griffiths, D., Hugh, M., Landers, K., & Lagli, N. (1996). Childhood play and adolescent leisure preferences: Associations with gender typing and the pre sence of siblings. Sex Roles, 35, 233-245.

    Google Scholar 

  • DiPietro, J. A. (1981). Rough and tumble play: A function of gender. Developmental Psychology, 17, 50-58.

    Google Scholar 

  • Etaugh, C., & Liss, M. B. (1992). Home, school, and playroom: Training grounds for adult gender roles. Sex Roles, 26, 129-147.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fabes, R. A. (1994). Physiological, emotional, and behavioral correlates of gender segregation. In C. Leaper (Ed.), The development of gender and relationships. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fabes, R. A., Shepard, S. A., Guthrie, I. K. & Martin, C. L. (1997). Roles of temperame ntal arousal and gender-se gregated play in young children's social adjustment. Developmental Psychology, 33, 693-702.

    Google Scholar 

  • Feiring, C., & Lewis, M. (1991). The deve lopment of social networks from early to middle childhood: Gender differences and the relation to school competence. Sex Roles, 25, 237-253.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fishbein, H. D., & Imai, S. (1993). Preschoolers sele ct playmates on the basis of gender and race. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 14, 303-316.

    Google Scholar 

  • Funk, J. B., & Buchman, D. D. (1996). Children's perceptions of gender differences in social approval for playing electronic games. Sex Roles, 35, 219-231.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hartup, W. W., French, D. C., Laursen, B., Johnston, M. K., & Ogawa, J. R. (1993). Conflict and friendship relations in middle childhood: Behavior in a close-field situation. Child Development, 64, 445-454.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hayden-Thomson, L., Rubin, K. H., & Hymel, S. (1987). Sex preference s in sociometric choices. Developmental Psychology, 23, 558-562.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hoffmann, M. L., & Powlishta, K. K. (1997, April). The social con text of gender segregation. Poster presented at the biennial meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, Washington, DC.

  • Howes, C. (1988). Same-and cross-sex friends: Implications for interaction and social skills. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 3, 21-37.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jacklin, C. N., & Maccoby, E. E. (1978). Social behavior at 33 months in same-sex and mixedsex dyads. Child Development, 49, 557-569.

    Google Scholar 

  • King, W. C., Jr., Miles, E. W., & Kniska, J. (1991). Boys will be boys (and girls will be girls): The attribution of gender role stereotypes in a gaming situation. Sex Roles, 25, 607-623.

    Google Scholar 

  • LaFreniere, P., Strayer, F. F., & Gauthier, R. (1984). The emergence of same-sex preferences among preschool peers. Child Development, 55, 1958-1966.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leaper C. (Ed.) (1994a). Childhood gender segregation: Causes and consequences. New directions for child development, no. 65. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leaper, C. (1994b). Exploring the consequences of gender segregation on social relationships. In C. Leaper (Ed.), Childhood gender segregation: Causes and consequences. New directions for child development, no. 65. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lever, J. (1976). Sex differences in the games children play. Social Problems, 23, 478-487.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maccoby, E. E. (1988). Gender as a social category. Developmental Psychology, 24, 755-765.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maccoby, E. E., & Jacklin, C. N. (1987). Gender segre gation in childhood. In E. H. Reese (Ed.), Advances in child development and behavior (Vol. 20). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maltz, D. N., & Borker, R. A. (1983). A cultural approach to male-female miscommunication. In J. A. Gumperz (Ed.), Language and social identity. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller, P. M., Danaher, D. L., & Forbes, D. (1986). Sex-related strategies for coping with interpersonal conflict in children aged five to seven. Developmental Psychology, 22, 543-548.

    Google Scholar 

  • Neppl, T. K., & Murray, A. D. (1997). Social dominance and play patterns among preschoolers: Gender comparisons. Sex Roles, 36, 381-393.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pellegrini, A. D. (1989). Elementary school children's rough-and-tumble play. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 4, 245-260.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pellegrini, A. D., & Davis, P. D. (1993). Relations between children's playground and classroom behavior. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 63, 88-95.

    Google Scholar 

  • Poest, C. A., Williams, J. R., Witt, D. D., & Atwood, M. E. (1989). Physical activity patterns of pre school children. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 4, 367-376.

    Google Scholar 

  • Powlishta, K. K. (1995). Intergroup processes in childhood: Social categorization and sex role development. Developmental Psychology, 31, 781-788.

    Google Scholar 

  • Powlishta, K. K., Serbin, L. A., & Moller, L. C. (1993). The stability of individual differences in gender-typing: Implications for understanding gender segregation. Sex Roles, 29, 723-737.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rotenberg, K. J. (1984). Sex differences in children's trust in peers. Sex Roles, 11, 953-957.

    Google Scholar 

  • Serbin, L. A., Powlishta, K. K., & Gulko, J. (1993). The development of sex typing in middle childhood. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 58(2, Serial 232). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Serbin, L. A., & Sprafkin, C. (1986). The salience of gender and the process of sex-typing in three-to seven-year-old children. Child Development, 57, 1188-1199.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sheldon, A. (1990). Pickle fights: Gendered talk in preschool disputes. Discourse Processes, 13, 5-31.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thorne, B. (1986). Girls and boys together, but mostly apart. In W. W. Hartup & Z. Rubin (Eds.), Relationships and development. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thorne, B. (1993). Gender play: Girls and boys in school. New Brunswick, NJ: Rutgers University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tricerri, M. F., & Benenson, J. F. (1997, April). Group versus dyadic interaction in boys and girls. Poster presented at the biennial meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, Washington, DC.

  • Unger, R., & Crawford, M. (1992). Women an d gender: A feminist psychology. New York: McGraw-Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Waldrop, M. F., & Halverson, F., Jr. (1975). Intensive and extensive peer behavior: Longitudinal and cross-sectional analysis. Child Development, 46, 19-26.

    Google Scholar 

  • Whiting, B. B., & Edwards, C. P. (1988). Children of different worlds. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zarbatany, L., Hartmann, D. P., & Rankin, D. B. (1990). The psychological functions of preadolescent peer activities. Child Development, 61, 1067-1080.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Boyatzis, C.J., Mallis, M. & Leon, I. Effects of Game Type on Children's Gender-Based Peer Preferences: A Naturalistic Observational Study. Sex Roles 40, 93–105 (1999). https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1018882417926

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1018882417926

Keywords

Navigation