Abstract
A secondary-task probe (tone) was presentedintermittently while men viewed erotic film segmentsacross a session involving 18 trials with the same filmsegment (habituation), then 2 trials with different film segments (novelty) and 2 trials withreinstatement of the original segment (dishabituation).Reaction time to the tone (an index of the extentprocessing resources were being committed to the erotic stimulus) shifted during the session inparallel with changes that occurred in penile tumescenceand subjective sexual arousal. The decrease in sexualarousal over the first 18 trials in the session was accompanied by a progressively faster reactionto the tone, novel stimulation led to recovery of sexualarousal and a slower reaction to the tone, and on trials21 and 22 sexual arousal and reaction time levels were above the values that prevailedimmediately prior to novel stimulation. Results arediscussed with reference to the relationship betweenhabituation and attention.
Similar content being viewed by others
REFERENCES
Arntz, A., Dreesen, L., and Merckelbach, H. (1991). Attention, not anxiety, influences pain. Behav. Res. Ther. 29: 41–50.
Bancroft, J. H., Jones, H. C., and Pullan, B. R. (1966). A single transducer for measuring penile erection with comments on its use in the treatment of sexual disorders. Behav. Res. Ther. 4: 239–241.
Barlow, D. H. (1986). The causes of sexual dysfunction: The role of anxiety and cognitive interference. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol.. 54: 140–148.
Bentler, P. M. (1968). Heterosexual behavior assessment—1. Males. Behav. Res. Ther. 6: 21–25.
Dawson, M. R. (1990). Psychophysiology at the interface of clinical science, cognitive science, and neuroscience. Psychophysiology 27: 243–255.
Dawson, M. R., Filion, D. L., and Schell, A. M. (1989). Is elicitation of the autonomic orienting response associated with allocation of processing resources? Psychophysiology 26: 560–572.
Dawson, M. R., Schell, A. M., and Hazlett, E. A. (1991). The relationship between skin conductance orienting and the allocation of processing resources. Psychophysiology 28: 410–424.
Dekker, J., and Everaerd, W. (1989). Psychological determinants of sexual arousal: A review. Behav. Res. Ther. 27: 353–364.
Drummond, F., White, K., and Ashton, R. (1978). Imagery vividness affects habituation rate. Psychophysiology 15: 193–195.
Farkas, G. M., Sine, L. F., and Evans, I. M. (1979). The effects of distraction, performance demand, stimulus explicitness, and personality on objective and subjective measures of male sexual arousal. Behav. Res. Ther. 17: 25–32.
Geer, J. H., and Fuhr, R. (1976). Cognitive factors in sexual arousal: The role of distraction. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 44: 238–243.
Julien, E., and Over, R. (1984). Male sexual arousal with repeated exposure to erotic stimuli. Arch. Sex. Behav. 13: 211–222.
Koukounas, E., and Over, R. (1993). Habituation and dishabituation of male sexual arousal. Behav. Res. Ther. 31: 575–585.
Laan, E., and Everaerd, W. (1995). Habituation of female sexual arousal to slides and. lm. Arch. Sex. Behav. 24: 517–541.
Lang, P. J., Greenwald, M. K., Bradley, M. M., and Hamm, A. O. (1993). Looking at pictures: Affective, facial, visceral, and behavioral reactions. Psychophysiology 30: 261–273.
Meuwissen, I., and Over, R. (1990). Habituation and dishabituation of female sexual arousal. Behav. Res. Ther. 28: 217–226.
O'Donohue, W. T., and Geer, J. H. (1985). The habituation of sexual arousal. Arch. Sex. Behav. 14: 233–246.
O'Donohue, W., and Plaud, J. J. (1991). The long-term habituation of sexual arousal in the human male. J. Behav. Ther. Exp. Psychiatry 22: 87–96.
O'Gorman, J. G. (1977). Individual differences in habituation of human physiological responses: A review of theory, method, and. ndings in the study of personality correlates in non-clinical populations. Biol. Psychol. 5: 257–318.
Ohman, A. (1979). The orienting response, attention, and learning: An information-processing perspective. In Kimmel, H. D., van Olst, E. H., and Orlebeke, J. F. (eds.), The Orienting Reflex in Humans. Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ, pp. 443–471.
Over, R., and Koukounas, E. (1995). Habituation of sexual arousal: Product and process. Annu. Rev. Sex Res. 6: 187–223.
Posner, M. I., and Boies, S. J. (1971). Components of attention. Psychol. Rev. 78: 391–408.
Siddle, D. A. T., and Jordan, J. (1993). Effects of intermodality change on electrodermal orienting and on the allocation of processing resources. Psychophysiology 30: 429–436.
Siddle, D. A. T., and Spinke, J. A. (1991). Orienting, habituation, and the allocation of processing resources. In Campbell, B., Richardson, R., and Hayne, H. (eds.), Attention and Information Processing in Infants and Adults: Perspectives from Human and Animal Research, Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ, pp. 227–262.
Smith, D., and Over, R. (1987). Does fantasy-induced sexual arousal habituate? Behav. Res. Ther. 25: 477–485.
Tellegen, A., and Atkinson, G. (1974). Openness to absorbing and self-altering experiences ("absorption"), a trait related to hypnotic susceptibility. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 83: 268–277.
Rights and permissions
About this article
Cite this article
Koukounas, E., Over, R. Allocation of Attentional Resources During Habituation and Dishabituation of Male Sexual Arousal. Arch Sex Behav 28, 539–552 (1999). https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1018769200845
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1018769200845