Abstract
The article reviews studies that explain the role of graphical displays in learning and synthesizes relevant findings into principles for effective graphical design. Three theoretical perspectives provide the framework that organizes the review: dual coding theory, visual argument, and conjoint retention. The three theories are compatible although they are based on different assumptions. Research suggests that graphics are effective learning tools only when they allow readers to interpret and integrate information with minimum cognitive processing. Learners' characteristics, such as prior subject-matter knowledge, visuospatial ability, and strategies, influence graphic processing and interact with graphical design to mediate its effects. Future research should investigate the interplay between display and learner characteristics and how graphical design can address individual differences in learning from graphics.
This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution.
REFERENCES
Anderson, J. R. (1995). Cognitive Psychology and its Implications, Freeman, New York
Atkinson, R. K., Levin, J. R., Kiewra, K. A., Meyers, T., Kim, S. I., Atkinson, L. A., Renandya, W. A., and Hwang, Y. (1999). Matrix and mnemonic text processing adjuncts: Comparing and combining their components. J. Educ. Psychol. 91(2): 342–357
Ausubel, D. P. (1960). The use of advance organizers in the learning and retention of meaningful verbal material. J. Educ. Psychol. 51(5): 267–272
Baddeley, A. D., and Logie, R. H. (1999). Working memory: The multiple component model. In Miyake, A., and Shah, P. (eds.), Models of Working Memory: Mechanisms of Active Maintenance and Executive Control, Cambridge University Press, New York
Bauer, M. I., and Johnson-Laird, P. N. (1993). How diagrams can improve reasoning. Psychol. Sci. 4: 372–378
Bertin, J. (1983). Semiology of Graphics, The University ofWisconsin Press, Wisconsin
Brown, J. S., Collins, A., and Duguid, P. (1989). Situated cognition and the culture of learning. Educ. Res. 18(1): 32–42
Carroll, J.B. (1993). Human Cognitive Abilities:ASurvey of Factor-Analytic Studies, Cambridge University Press, New York
Clark, J. M., and Paivio, A. (1991). Dual coding theory and education. Educ. Psychol. Rev. 3(3): 149–210
Cox, R. (1999). Representation construction, externalized cognition and individual differences. Learn. Instruc. 9: 343–363
D'Esposito, M., Detre, J. A., Aguirre, G. K., Stallcup, M., Alsop, D. C., Tippet, L. J., and Farah, M. J. (1997). A functional MRI study of mental image generation. Neuropsychologia, 35(5): 725–730
Dickson, L. A. S., Schrankel, P. S., and Kulhavy, R. (1988). Verbal and spatial encoding of text. Instruc. Sci. 17: 145–157
Dunston, P. J. (1992). A critique of graphic organizer research. Read. Res. Instruc. 31(2): 57–65
Finke, R. A., and Shepard, R. N. (1986). Visual functions of mental imagery. In Boff, K. R., Kaufman, L., and Thomas, J. P. (eds.), Handbook of Perception and Human Performance, Wiley, New York
Gerber, R., Boulton-Lewis, G., and Bruce, C. (1995). Children's understanding of graphic representations of quantitative data. Learn. Instruc. 5: 77–100
Goodman, N. (1968). The Languages of Art, Hackett, Indianapolis
Griffin, M. M., and Robinson, D. H. (2000). Role of mimeticism and spatiality in textual recall. Contemp. Educ. Psychol. 25: 125–149
Guthrie, J. T., Weber, S., and Kimmerly, N. (1993). Searching documents: Cognitive processes and deficits in understanding graphs, tables, and illustrations. Contemp. Educ. Psychol. 18: 186–221
Hawk, P. P. (1986). Using graphic organizers to increase achievement in middle school life science. Sci. Educ. 70(1): 81–87
Hegarty, M., Carpenter, P. A., and Just, M. A. (1991). Diagrams in the comprehension of scientific text. In Barr, R., Kamil, M. L., Mosenthal, P. B., and Pearson, P. D. (eds.), Handbook of Reading Research, Vol. 2, Longman, New York
Hegarty, M., and Just, M. A. (1989). Understanding machines from text and diagrams. In Mandl, H., and Levin, J. R. (eds.), Knowledge Acquisition From Text and Pictures, Elsevier Science, New York
Hegarty, M., and Just, M. A. (1993). Constructing mental models from text and diagrams. J. Mem. Lang. 32: 717–742
Johnson-Laird, P. N. (1998). Imagery, visualization, and thinking. In Hochberg, J. (ed.), Perception and Cognition at Century's End. Handbook of Perception and Cognition, Academic Press, New York
Jonides, J., and Smith, E. E. (1997). The architecture of working memory. In Rugg, M. D. (ed.), Cognitive Neuroscience, Taylor & Francis, London
Kenny, R. F. (1995). The generative effects of instructional organizers with computer-based interactive video. J. Educ. Comput. Res. 12(3): 275–296
Kiewra, K. A., Robinson, D. H., Christian, D., and McShane, A. (1988). Providing study notes: Relation of three types of notes for review. J. Educ. Psychol. 80: 595–597
Kosslyn, S. M. (1981). The medium and the message in mental imagery: A theory. Psychol. Rev. 88(1): 46–66
Kosslyn, S. M. (1988). Imagery in learning. In Gazzaniga, M. S. (ed.), Perspectives in Memory Research, MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, pp. 245–273
Kosslyn, S. M. (1989). Understanding charts and graphs. Appl. Cogn. Psychol. 3: 185–226
Kosslyn, S. M. (1994). Image and Brain. The Resolution of the Imagery Debate, The MIT Press, Cambridge, MA.
Kozma, R. B. (1991). Learning with Media. Rev. Educ. Res. 61(2): 179–211
Kulhavy, R. W., Stock, W. A., and Caterino, L. C. (1994). Reference maps as a framework for remembering text. In Schnotz, W., and Kulhavy, R.W. (eds.), Comprehension of Graphics, Elsevier Science, New York, pp. 153–162
Kulhavy, R. W., Stock, W. A., and Kealy, W. A. (1993a). How geographic maps increase recall of instructional text. Educ. Technol. Res. Dev. 41(4): 47–62
Kulhavy, R. W., Stock, W. A., Peterson, S. E., Pridemore, D. R., and Klein, J. D. (1992). Using maps to retrieve text: A test of conjoint retention. Contemp. Educ. Psychol. 17: 56–70
Kulhavy, R. W., Stock, W. A., Woodard, K. A., and Haygood, R. C. (1993b). Comparing elaboration and dual coding theories: The case of maps and text. Contemp. Educ. Psychol. 106(4): 483–498
Kulhavy, R. W., Woodard, K. A., Haygood, R. C., and Webb, J. M. (1993c). Using maps to remember text: An instructional analysis. Br. J. Educ. Psychol. 63: 161–169
Lambiotte, J. G., Dansereau, D. F., Cross, D. R., and Reynolds, S. B. (1989). Multirelational semantic maps. Educ. Psychol. Rev. 1(4): 331–367
Larkin, J. H., and Simon, H. A. (1987). Why a diagram is (sometimes) worth ten thousand words. Cognitive Sci. 11: 65–99
Levin, J. R., Anglin, G. J., and Carney, R. N. (1987). On empirically validating functions of pictures in prose. In Willows, D. M., and Houghton, H. A. (eds.), The Psychology of Illustration, Vol. 1, Springer-Verlag, New York, pp. 51–85
Levin, J. R., and Mayer, R. E. (1993). Understanding illustrations in text. In Britton, B. K., Woodward, A., and Binkley, M. (eds.), Learning from textbooks. Theory and practice (pp. 95–114). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates
Lewandowsky, S., and Behrens, J. T. (1999). Statistical maps and graphs. In Durso, F. T., Nickerson, R. S., Schvaneveldt, R. W., Dumais, S. T., Lindsay, D. S., Chi, M. T. H. (eds.). Handbook of Applied Cognition, Wiley, New York
Logie, R. H. (1995). Visuospatial Working Memory, Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ
Lohse, G., Walker, N., Biolsi, K., and Rueter, H. (1991). Classifying graphical information. Behav. Inf. Technol. 50(5): 419–436
Lowe, R. K. (1994). Selectivity in diagrams: Reading beyond the lines. Educ. Psychol. 14(4): 467–491
Lowe, R.K. (1996). Background knowledge and the construction of a situational representation from a diagram. Eur. J. Psychol. Educ. 11(4): 377–397
Mayer, R. E. (1989a). Models for understanding. Rev. Educ. Res. 59(1): 43–64
Mayer, R. E. (1989b). Systematic thinking fostered by illustrations in scientific text. J. Educ. Psychol. 81(2): 240–246
Mayer, R. E. (1994). Building mental representations from pictures and words. In Schnotz, W., and Kulhavy, R.W. (eds.), Comprehension of Graphics, North-Holland, Amsterdam
Mayer, R. E., and Anderson, R. B. (1991). Animations need narrations: An experimental test of a dual coding hypothesis. J. Educ. Psychol. 83: 484–490
Mayer, R. E., and Anderson, R. B. (1992). The instructive animation: Helping students build connections between words and pictures in multimedia learning. J. Educ. Psychol. 84: 444–452
Mayer, R. E., Bove, W., Bryman, A., Mars, R., and Tapanengco, L. (1996). When less is more: Meaningful learning from visual and verbal summaries of science textbook lessons. J. Educ. Psychol. 88(1): 64–73
Mayer, R. E., and Gallini, J. K. (1990). When is an illustration worth ten thousand words? J. Educ. Psychol. 82(4): 715–726
Mayer, R. E., and Moreno, R. (1998). A split-attention effect in multimedia learning: Evidence for dual processing systems in working memory. J. Educ. Psychol. 90(2): 312–320
Mayer, R. E., and Sims, V.K. (1994). For whom is a picture worth a thousand words? Extensions of a dual coding theory of multimedia learning. J. Educ. Psychol. 86(3): 389–401
Mayer, R. E., Steinhoff, K., Bower, G., and Mars, R. (1995). A generative theory of textbook design: Using annotated illustrations to foster meaningful learning of science text. Educ. Technol. Res. Dev. 43(1): 31–43
Miyake, A., and Shah, P. (1999). Toward unified theories of working memory: Emerging general consensus, unresolved theoretical issues, and future research directions. In Miyake, A., and Shah, P. (eds.), Models of Working Memory: Mechanisms of Active Maintenance and Executive Control, Cambridge University Press, New York
Mokros, J. R., and Tinker, R. F. (1987). The impact of microcomputer-based labs on children's ability to interpret graphs. J. Res. Sci. Teaching 24(4): 369–383
Moreno, R., and Mayer, R. E. (1999). Cognitive principles of multimedia learning: The role of modality and contiguity. J. Educ. Psychol. 91(2): 358–368
Mousavi, S. Y., Low, R., and Sweller, J. (1995). Reducing cognitive load by mixing auditory and visual presentation modes. J. Educ. Psychol. 87: 319–334
Narayanan, N. H., Suwa, M., and Motoda, H. (1995). Hypothesizing behaviors from device diagrams. In Glasgow, J., Narayanan, N. H., and Chandrasekaran, B. (eds.), Diagrammatic Reasoning: Cognitive and Computational Perspectives, AAAI Press, Menlo Park, CA.
National Research Council (1996). National Science Education Standards, National Academy Press, Washington, DC.
Novak, J. D. (1996). Concept mapping: A tool for improving science teaching and learning. In Treagust, D. F., Duit, R., and Fraser, B. J. (eds.), Improving teaching and learning in science and mathematics. New York: Teachers College Press
O'Donnell, A. (1993). Searching for information in knowledge maps and texts. Contemp. Educ. Psychol. 18: 222–239
Paivio, A. (1971). Imagery and Verbal Processes, Holt, Rinehart, and Winston, New York
Paivio, A. (1983). The empirical case for dual coding. In Yuille, J. (ed.), Imagery, Cognition, and Memory, Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ.
Paivio, A. (1990). Mental Representations. A Dual Coding Approach, Oxford University Press, New York
Paivio, A., Clark, J. M., and Khan, M. (1988). Effects on concreteness and semantic relatedness on composite imagery ratings and cued recall. Mem. Cogn. 16(5): 422–430
Paivio, A., and Csapo, K. (1973). Picture superiority in free recall: Imagery or dual coding? Cogn. Psychol. 5: 176–206
Paivio, A., Walsh, M., and Bons, T. (1994). Concreteness effects on memory: When and why? J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 20(5): 1196–1204
Pea, R. (1994). Seeing what we build together: Distributed multimedia learning environments for transformative communications. J. Learn. Sci. 3(3): 285–299
Purnell, K. N., Solman, R. T., and Sweller, J. (1991). The effects of technical illustrations on cognitive load. Instruc. Sci. 20: 443–462
Pylyshyn, Z.W. (1973). What the mind's eye tells the mind's brain:Acritique of mental imagery. Psychol. Bull. 80: 1–24
Pylyshyn, Z.W. (1981). The imagery debate: Analogue media versus tacit knowledge. Psychol. Rev. 87: 16–45
Reisberg, D., and Heuer, F. (in press). Visuospatial images. In Shah, P., and Miyake, A. (eds.), Handbook of Visuospatial Thinking, Cambridge University Press, New York
Rewey, K. L., Danserau, D. F., and Peel, J. L. (1991). Knowledge maps and information processing strategies. Contemp. Educ. Psychol. 16: 203–214
Rice, G. E. (1994). Need for explanations in graphic organizer research. Read. Psychol. 15(1): 39–67
Rieber, L. (1990a). Animation in computer-based instruction. Educ. Technol. Res. Dev. 38(1): 77–86
Rieber, L. P. (1990b). Using computer-animated graphics in science instruction with children. J. Educ. Psychol. 82(1): 135–140
Rieber, L. P. (1991a). Animation, incidental learning, and continuing motivation. J. Educ. Psychol. 83(3): 318–328
Rieber, L. P. (1991b). Effects of visual grouping strategies of computer-animated presentations on selective attention in science. Educ. Technol. Res. Dev. 39(4): 5–15
Rittschof, K. A., and Kulhavy, R. W. (1998). Learning and remembering from thematic maps of familiar regions. Educ. Technol. Res. Dev. 46(1): 19–38
Rittschof, K. A., Stock, W. A., Kulhavy, R.W., Verdi, M. P., and Doran, J. M. (1994). Thematic maps improve memory for facts and inferences: A test of the stimulus order hypothesis. Contemp. Educ. Psychol. 19(2): 129–142
Robinson, D. H. (1998). Graphic organizers as aids to text learning. Read. Res. Instruc. 37: 85–105
Robinson, D. H., Katayama, A. D., DuBois, N. F., and Devaney, T. (1998). Interactive effects of graphic organizers and delayed review of concept application. J. Exp. Educ. 67(1): 17–31
Robinson, D. H., Katayama, A. D., and Fan, A.-C. (1996). Evidence for conjoint retention of information encoded from spatial adjunct displays. Contemp. Educ. Psychol. 21: 221–239
Robinson, D. H., and Kiewra, K. A. (1995). Visual argument: Graphic organizers are superior to outlines in improving learning from text. J. Educ. Psychol. 87(3): 455–467
Robinson, D. H., and Molina, E. (2002). The relative involvement of visual and auditory working memory when studying adjunct displays. Contemp. Educ. Psychol. 27(1): 118–131
Robinson, D. H., Robinson, S. L., and Katayama, A. D. (1999). When words are represented in memory like pictures: Evidence for the spatial encoding of study materials. Contemp. Educ. Psychol. 24: 38–54
Robinson, D. H., and Schraw, G. (1994). Computational efficiency through visual argument: Do graphic organizers communicate relations in text too effectively? Contemp. Educ. Psychol. 19: 399–415
Robinson, D. H., and Skinner, C. H. (1996). Why graphic organizers facilitate search processes: Fewer words or computationally efficient indexing? Contemp. Educ. Psychol. 21: 166–180
Sadoski, M., Goetz, E. T., and Fritz, J. B. (1993). Impact of concreteness on comprehensibility, interest, and memory for text: Implications for dual coding theory and text design. J. Educ. Psychol. 85(2): 291–304
Scaife, M., and Rogers, Y. (1996). External cognition: How do graphical representations work? Int. J. Hum. Comput. Stud. 45: 185–213
Scevak, J. J., Moore, P. J., and Kirby, J. R. (1993). Training students to use maps to increase text recall. Contemp. Educ. Psychol. 18: 401–413
Schwartz, N. H., Ellsworth, L. S., Graham, L., and Knight, B. (1998). Accessing prior knowledge to remember text:Acomparison of advance organizers and maps. Contemp. Educ. Psychol. 23: 65–89
Schwartz, N. H., and Philippe, A. E. (1991). Individual differences in the retention of maps. Contemp. Educ. Psychol. 16: 171–182
Schwartz, N. H., and Wilkinson, W. K. (1992). Map-passage structural hierarchy and passage recall. Contemp. Educ. Psychol. 17: 356–336
Shah, P., and Carpenter, P. A. (1995). Conceptual limitations in comprehending line graphs. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 124(1): 43–61
Shah, P., Mayer, R. E., and Hegarty, M. (1999). Graphs as aids to knowledge construction: Signaling techniques for guiding the process of graph comprehension. J. Educ. Psychol. 91(4): 690–702
Shah, P., and Miyake, A. (1996). The separability of working memory resources for spatial thinking and language processing: An individual differences approach. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 125(1): 4–27
Simmons, D. (1988). Effects of teacher-constructed pre-and post-graphic organizer instruction on sixth-grade science students' comprehension and recall. J. Educ. Res. 82(1): 15–21
Stock, W. A., Kulhavy, R. W., Peterson, S. E., Hancock, T. E., and Verdi, M. P. (1995). Mental representations of maps and verbal descriptions: Evidence they may affect text memory differently. Contemp. Educ. Psychol. 20: 237–256
Sweller, J., and Chandler, P. (1994). Why is some material difficult to learn. Cogn. Instruc. 12(3): 185–233
Sweller, J., Chandler, P., Tierney, P., and Cooper, M. (1990). Cognitive load as a factor in the structuring of technical material. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 119(2): 176–192
Sweller, J., van Merrienboer, J. J. G., and Paas, F. G. W. C. (1998). Cognitive architecture and instructional design. Educ. Psychol. Rev. 10(3): 251–296
Tukey, J. W. (1990). Data-based graphics: Visual displays in the decades to come. Stat. Sci. 5: 327–329
Tversky, B. (2001). Spatial schemas in depictions. In Gattis, M. (ed.), Spatial Schemas and Abstract Thought, MIT Press, Cambridge, MA.
Tversky, B. (1995). Cognitive origins of graphic productions. In Marchese, F. T. (ed.), Understanding Images, Springer-Verlag, New York
Verdi, M. P., Johnson, J. T., Stock, W. A., Kulhavy, R. W., and Whitman-Ahern, P. (1997). Organized spatial displays and texts: Effects of presentation order and display type on learning outcomes. J. Exp. Educ. 65(4): 303–317
Verdi, M. P., Kulhavy, R. W., Stock, W. A., Rittschof, K. A., and Johnson, J. T. (1996). Text learning using scientific diagrams: Implications for classroom use. Contemp. Educ. Psychol. 21: 487–499
Waller, R. (1981). Understanding network diagrams. Paper presented at the Annual Meeting of the American Educational Research Association, Los Angeles, April 1981
Wiegmann, D. A., Dansereau, D. F., McCagg, E. C., Rewey, K. L., and Pitre, U. (1992). Effects of knowledge map characteristics on information processing. Contemp. Educ. Psychol. 17: 136–155
Willerman, M., and Harg, R. A. M. (1991). The concept map as an advance organizer. J. Res. Sci. Teaching 28(8): 705–711
Winn, W. (1987). Charts, graphs, and diagrams in educational materials. In Willows, D. M., and Houghton, H. A. (eds.), The Psychology of Illustration, Vol. 1, Springer-Verlag, New York, pp. 152–198
Winn, W. (1991). Learning from maps and diagrams. Educ. Psychol. Rev. 3(3): 211–247
Winn, W. (1994). Contributions of perceptual and cognitive processes to the comprehension of graphics. In Schnotz, W., and Kulhavy, R.W. (eds.), Comprehension of Graphics, Elsevier Science, New York
Winn, W., Li, T.-Z., and Schill, D. (1991). Diagrams as aids to problem solving: Their role in facilitating search and computation. Educ. Technol. Res. Dev. 39(1): 17–29
Zacks, J., and Tversky, B. (1999). Bars and lines: A study of graphic communication. Mem. Cogn. 27(6): 1073–1079
Zhang, J. (1997). The nature of external representations in problem solving. Cogn. Sci. 21(2): 179–217
Zhang, J., and Norman, D. A. (1994). Representations in distributed cognitive tasks. Cogn. Sci. 18: 87–122.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Rights and permissions
About this article
Cite this article
Vekiri, I. What Is the Value of Graphical Displays in Learning?. Educational Psychology Review 14, 261–312 (2002). https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1016064429161
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1016064429161