Skip to main content
Log in

Organizing Components into Combinations: How Stage Transition Works

  • Published:
Journal of Adult Development Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

This paper investigates the nature of transition between stages. The Model of Hierarchical Complexity of tasks leads to a quantal notion of stage, and therefore delineates the nature of stage transition. Piaget's dialectical model of stage change was extended and precisely specified. Transition behavior was shown to consist of alternations in previous-stage behavior. As transition proceeded, the alternations increased in rate until the previous stage behaviors were “smashed” together. Once the smashed-together pieces became coordinated, new-stage behavior could be said to have formed. Because stage transition is quantal, individuals can only change performance by whole stage. We reviewed theories of the specific means by which new-stage behavior may be acquired and the emotions and personalities associated with steps in transition. Contemporary challenges in the society increasingly call for transition to postformal and postconventional responses on the part of both individuals and institutions as the examples illustrate.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

REFERENCES

  • Arlin, P. K. (1975). Cognitive development in adulthood: A fifth stage? Developmental Psychology, 11, 602-606.

    Google Scholar 

  • Arlin, P. K. (1977). Piagetian operations in problem finding. Developmental Psychology, 13, 247-298.

    Google Scholar 

  • Arlin, P. K. (1984). Adolescent and adult thought: A structural interpretation. In M. L. Commons, F.A. Richards, & C. Armon (Eds.), Beyond formal operations: Vol. 1. Late adolescent and adult cognitive development. New York: Praeger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Armon, C. (1984). Ideals of the good life and moral judgment: Ethical reasoning across the life span. In M. L. Commons, F.A. Richards, & C. Armon (Eds.), Beyond formal operations: Vol. 1. Late adolescent and adult cognitive development (pp. 357-380). New York: Praeger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Armon, C., & Dawson, T. L. (1997). Developmental trajectories in moral reasoning across the life span. Journal of Moral Education, 26(4), 433-453.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baer, D. M., & Rosales, J. (1994, May). Development cusps: A relevant concept for behavior analysis. Paper presented at the Association for Behavior Analysis, Atlanta, GA.

  • Basseches, M. A. (1984). Dialectical thinking as a metasystematic form of cognitive organization. In M. L. Commons, F. A. Richards, & C. Armon (Eds.), Beyond formal operations: Vol. 1. Late adolescent and adult cognitive development (pp. 216-238). New York: Praeger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Benack, S., & Basseches, M. A. (1989). Dialectical thinking and relativistic epistemology: Their relation in adult development. In M. L. Commons, J. D. Sinnott, F. A. Richards, & C. Armon (Eds.), Adult Development: Vol. 1. Comparisons and applications of adolescent and adult developmental models (pp. 95-132). New York: Praeger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bidell, T. R., & Fischer, K. W. (1992). Beyond the stage debate: Action, structure, and variability in Piagetian theory and research. In, C. A. Berg & R. J. Sternberg (Eds.), Intellectual development. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Binder, C. (2000). Component/composite analysis and programming from the “real world.” Paper presented at the association for Behavior Analysis,Washington, DC.

  • Bouchard, T. J., Jr. (1997). IQ similarity in twins reared apart: Findings and responses to critics. In R. J. Sternberg & E. L. Grigorenko (Eds.), Intelligence, heredity, and environment (pp. 126-160). New York: Cambridge University.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bouchard, T. J., Lykken, D. T., McGue, M., & Segal, N. L. (1990). Sources of human psychological differences: The Minnesota Study of Twins Reared Apart. Science, 250(4978), 223-228.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bouchard, T. J., Lykken, D. T., McGue, M., & Segal, N. L. (1991). Sources of human psychological differences: The Minnesota Study of Twins Reared Apart: Response. Science, 252(5003), 191-192.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brendel, J. M., Kolbert, J. B., & Foster, V. A. (2002). Promoting student cognitive development. Journal of Adult Development.

  • Colby, A., & Kohlberg, L. (1987). The measurement of moral judgement: Vol. 1. Theoretical foundations and research validation. New York: Cambridge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Commons, M. L., Danaher, D., & Meaney, M. (2002, June). Transition and stage in performance of Harvard University faculty and staff. Data presented at Society for Research in Adult Development Symposium, Pace University, New York City, New York.

  • Commons, M. L., Danaher, D. L., Miller, P. M., Goodheart, E. A., Dawson, T. L. with Johnstone, J., Straughn, J. B., Weaver, J. H., Lichtenbaum, E., Krause, S. R., & Broderick, M. A. (2000). Hierarchical Complexity Scoring System (HCSS):Howto score

  • Commons, M. L., & Miller, P. M. (1998). A quantitative behavioranalytic theory of development. Mexican Journal of Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 24(2), 153-180.

    Google Scholar 

  • Commons, M. L., & Richards, F. A. (1984a). A general model of stage theory. In M. L. Commons, F. A. Richards, & C. Armon (Eds.), Beyond formal operations: Vol. 1. Late adolescent and adult cognitive development (pp. 120-140). NewYork: Praeger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Commons, M. L., & Richards, F. A. (1984b). Applying the general stage model. In M. L. Commons, F. A. Richards, & C. Armon (Eds.), Beyond formal operations: Vol. 1. Late adolescent and adult cognitive development (pp. 141-157). NewYork: Praeger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Commons, M. L., & Richards, F. A. (1995). Behavior analytic approach to dialectics of stage performance and stage change. Behavioral Development, 5(2), 7-9.

    Google Scholar 

  • Commons, M. L., Trudeau, E. J., Stein, S. A., Richards, F. A., & Krause, S. R. (1998). The existence of developmental stages as shown by the hierarchical complexity of tasks. Developmental Review, 8(3), 237-278.

    Google Scholar 

  • Draney, K. L. (1996). The polytomous Saltus model: A mixture model approach to the diagnosis of developmental differences. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of California at Berkeley. Berkeley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fischer, K. W. (1980). A theory of cognitive development: The control and construction of hierarchies of skills. Psychological Review, 87, 477-531.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fischer, K. W., Hand, H. H., & Russell, S. (1984). The development of abstractions in adolescents and adulthood. In M. L. Commons, F.A. Richards, & C. Armon (Eds.), Beyond formal operations: Late adolescent and adult cognitive development (pp. 43-73). New York: Praeger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fischer, K.W., & Kenny, S. L. (1986). Environmental conditions for discontinuities in the development of abstractions. In R. A. Mines & K. S. Kitchener (Eds.), Adult cognitive development: Methods and models (pp. 57-75). New York: Praeger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fischer, K. W., & Lazerson, A.(1984). Human development: From conception through adolescence. New York: Freeman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Flavell, J. H. (1971). Comments on Beilin's “The development of physical concepts.” In T. Mischel (Ed.), Cognitive development and epistemology (pp. 121-128). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gewirtz, J. L. (1969). Mechanisms of social learning: Some roles of stimulation and behavior in early human development. In D. A. Goslin (Ed.), Handbook of socialization theory and research (pp. 57-212). Chicago: Rand McNally.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grotzer, T. A., McCarthy, K. G., Frome, K.W., & Commons, M. L. (June, 1987). The process of stage change. Paper presented at Beyond Formal Operations 3: Positive development during adolescence and adulthood. The Development of Adolescent and Adult Thought and Perception, Harvard University,Cambridge.

  • Herrnstein, R. J. (1982). Melioration as behavioral dynamism. In M. L. Commons, R. J. Herrnstein, & H. Rachlin (Eds.), Quantitative analyses of behavior: Vol. 2. Matching and maximizing accounts (pp. 433-458). Cambridge, MA: Ballinger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Herrnstein, R. J., & Vaughan, W., Jr. (1980). Melioration and behavioral allocation. In J. E. R. Staddon (Ed.), Limits to action: The allocation of individual behavior (pp. 143-176). NewYork: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Holland, J. G., & Skinner, B. F. (1961). The analysis of behavior: A program for self-instruction. New York: McGraw-Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lovell, C.W. (2002). Development and disequilibration: Predicting counselor trainee gain and loss scores on the Supervisee Levels Questionnaire. Journal of Adult Development, 9(3), 233-238.

    Google Scholar 

  • McAuliffe, G. J. (2002). Student changes, program influences, and adult development in one program of counselor training: An exploratory inductive inquiry. Journal of Adult Development, 9(3), 203-214.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller, P. M., Lee, S. T., & Commons, M. L. (2000, June). Scoring for stage and transition of losses in children and adult's lives. Data presented at Society for research in Adult Development Symposium, Pace University, New York City, New York.

  • Miller, P. M., & Lee, S. T. (2000, June). Stages and transitions in child and adult narratives about losses of attachment objects. Paper presented at the Jean Piaget Society, Montreal, Qu´ebec, Canada.

  • Mislevy, R. J., & Wilson, M. (1996). Marginal maximum likelihood estimation for a psychometric model of discontinuous development. Psychometrica, 61, 41-47.

    Google Scholar 

  • Moerk, E. L. (1992). Afirst language taught and learned. Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes.

    Google Scholar 

  • Moore, R., & Goldiamond, I. (1964). Errorless establishment of visual discrimination using fading procedures. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 7, 269-272.

    Google Scholar 

  • Overton, W. F. (1990). Reasoning, necessity, and logic: Developmental perspectives. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. (1954). The construction of reality in the child. (M. Cook, Trans.). New York: Ballantine Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J., Inhelder, B., & Sinclair-de Zwart, H. (1973). Memory and intelligence. A Pomerans (Trans.). New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Richards, F. A., & Commons, M. L. (1990). Applying signal detection theory to measure subject sensitivity to metasystematic, systematic and lower developmental stages. In M. L. Commons, C. Armon, L. Kohlberg, F. A. Richards, T. A. Grotzer, & J. D. Sinnott (Eds.), Adult Development: Vol 2. Models and methods in the study of adolescent and adult thought (pp. 175-188). New York: Praeger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Richard, D. C., Unger, C. M., & Commons, M. L. (1988, April 22). Strategies as knowledge: Subjects' methods of determining causality during the shift from concrete to formal operations. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the Eastern Psychological Association, Buffalo, NY. (Available from the Dare Institute, 234 Huron Avenue, Cambridge, MA 02138).

  • Riegel, K. F. (1973). Dialectic operations: The final phase of cognitive development. Human Development, 16, 346-370.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rosales-Ruiz, J. (May, 2000). Divide and teach. Presented at the Association for Behavior Analysis, Washington, DC.

  • Rosales-Ruiz, J., & Baer, D.M. (1997). Behavioral cusps: A developmental and pragmatic concept for behavior analysis. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 30(3), 533-544.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rosenberg, M. (1979). Conceiving the self. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Swan, T., & Benack, S. (2002). Narcissism in the epistemological pit. Journal of Adult Development, 9(3), 177-183.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sonnert, G., & Commons, M. L. (1994). Society and the highest stages of moral development. The Individual and Society, 4(1), 31-55.

    Google Scholar 

  • Terrace, H. S. (1961). Discrimination learning with and without “errors.” Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Harvard University, Cambridge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vygotsky,6L. S. (1966a). Language and thought. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vygotsky, L. S. (1966b). Development of the higher mental function. In Psychological research in the U.S.S.R. (pp. 44-45). Moscow: Progress Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, M. (1989). Saltus:Apsychometric model of discontinuity in cognitive development. Psychological Bulletin, 105,276-289.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wolfsont, C. (2000, June). The brain gym and scoring adult developmental transition and stage. Data presented at Society for research in Adult Development Symposium, Pace University, New York City, New York.

  • Wolfsont, C. (2002). Increasing behavioral skills and level of understanding in adults: A breif method integrating Dennison's BrainGymr balance with Piaget's reflective processes. Journal of Adult Development, 9(3), 185-201.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yan, Z. (2000, June). Always under construction: Dynamic variations in microdevelopmental transitions. Presented at the Piaget Society, Montreal, Qu´ebec, Canada.

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Michael Lamport Commons.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Commons, M.L., Richards, F.A. Organizing Components into Combinations: How Stage Transition Works. Journal of Adult Development 9, 159–177 (2002). https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1016047925527

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1016047925527

Navigation