Skip to main content
Log in

The Emotional Stroop Paradigm: Performance as a Function of Stimulus Properties and Self-Reported Mathematics Anxiety

  • Published:
Cognitive Therapy and Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Stroop tasks are considered useful in assessing anxious affect. Although symptomatically mathematics anxiety is similar to other anxiety conditions, demonstrating a Stroop interference effect among individuals with math anxiety has proven difficult. High and low math anxious individuals were administered a Stroop-like card task involving the counting of numerals and letters, along with a standard computer-based Stroop color-naming task using mathematical and neutral control words. On the card task, processing time was significantly longer among high math anxious participants, an effect most pronounced in the numeral condition. Response time did not differ on the standard Stroop color-naming task as a function of anxiety group or word type. Results suggest that interference effects of math anxiety may be a function of inhibitory deficits or excitatory processes (or both) that are compounded when exposed to more salient (i.e. numeric) stimuli. Implications regarding the assessment of mathematics anxiety and Stroop task methodology are discussed.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

REFERENCES

  • Ashcraft, M. H., & Faust, M. W. (1994). Mathematics anxiety and mental arithmetic performance: An exploratory investigation. Cognition and Emotion, 8, 97–125.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ashcraft, M. H., Kirk, E., & Hopko, D. R. (1998). On the cognitive consequences of mathematics anxiety. In C. Donlan (Ed.), The development of mathematical skills (pp. 175–199). London: UCL Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bush, G., Whalen, P. J., Rosen, B. R., Jenike, M. A., McInerney, S. C., & Rauch, S. L. (1998). The counting Stroop: An interference task specialized for functional neuroimaging - Validation study with functional MRI. Human Brain Mapping, 6, 270–282.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cassiday, K. L., McNally, R. J., & Zeitlin, S. B. (1992). Cognitive processing of trauma cues in rape victims with post-traumatic stress disorder. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 16, 283–295.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, J. D., Dunbar, K., & McClelland, J. L. (1990). On the control of automatic processes: A parallel distributed processing account of the Stroop effect. Psychological Review, 97, 332–361.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eysenck, M. W., & Calvo, M. G. (1992). Anxiety and performance: The processing efficiency theory. Cognition and Emotion, 6, 409–434.

    Google Scholar 

  • Foa, E.B., Ilai,D., McCarthy, P. R., Shoyer, B., & Murdock, T. (1993). Information processing in obsessive- compulsive disorder. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 17, 173–189.

    Google Scholar 

  • Freeman, T. J., & Lattal, K. A. (1992). Stimulus control of behavioral history. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 57, 5–15.

    Google Scholar 

  • Holle, C., Neely, J. H., & Heimberg, R. G. (1997). The effects of blocked versus random presentation and semantic relatedness of stimulus words on response to a modified Stroop task among social phobics. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 21, 681–697.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hopko, D. R., Ashcraft, M. H., Gute, J., Ruggiero, K. J., & Lewis, C. (1998). Mathematics anxiety and working memory: Support for the existence of a deficient inhibition mechanism. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 12, 343–355.

    Google Scholar 

  • MacLeod, C. M. (1991). Half a century of research on the Stroop effect: An integrative review. Psychological Bulletin, 109, 163–203.

    Google Scholar 

  • Masia, C. L., McNeil, D. W., Cohn, L. G., & Hope, D. A. (in press). Exposure to social anxiety words: Treatment for social phobia based on the Stroop paradigm. Cognitive and Behavioral Practice.

  • McLaughlin, M. S. (1996). Comparison of a self-report questionnaire and a Stroop color-naming task. Unpublished senior thesis. Oklahoma State University.

  • McNally, R. J., Kaspi, S. P., Riemann, B. C., & Zeitlin, S. B. (1990). Selective processing of threat cues in posttraumatic stress disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 99, 398–402.

    Google Scholar 

  • McNeil, D.W., Ries, B. J., Taylor, L. J., Boone, M. L., Carter, L. E., Turk, C. L., et al. (1995). Comparison of social phobia subtypes using Stroop tests. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 9, 47–57.

    Google Scholar 

  • Morton, J. (1969). Categories of interference:Verbal mediation and conflict in card sorting. British Journal of Psychology, 60, 329–346.

    Google Scholar 

  • Phaf, R. H., Christoffels, I. K., Waldorp, L. J., & den Dulk, P. (1998). Connectionist investigations of individual differences in Stroop performance. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 87, 899–914.

    Google Scholar 

  • Plake, B. S., & Parker, C. S. (1982).The development and validation of a revised version of the Mathematics Anxiety Rating Scale. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 42, 551–557.

    Google Scholar 

  • Richardson, F. C., & Suinn, R. M. (1972). The Mathematics Anxiety Rating Scale. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 19, 551–554.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spielberger, C. D. (1977). The Test Anxiety Inventory. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spielberger, C.D., Gorsuch, R., Lushene, R., Vagg, P. R., & Jacobs, G.A. (1983). Manual for the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory. Palo Alto: Consulting Psychologists Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stroop, J. R. (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 18, 643–661.

    Google Scholar 

  • Watson, D., & Friend, R. (1969). Measurement of social-evaluative anxiety. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 33, 448–457.

    Google Scholar 

  • Williams, J. M., Mathews, A., & MacLeod, C. (1996). The emotional Stroop task and psychopathology. Psychological Bulletin, 120, 3–24.

    Google Scholar 

  • Windes, J.D. (1968). Reaction t ime for numerical coding and naming of numerals. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 78, 318–322.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Hopko, D.R., McNeil, D.W., Gleason, P.J. et al. The Emotional Stroop Paradigm: Performance as a Function of Stimulus Properties and Self-Reported Mathematics Anxiety. Cognitive Therapy and Research 26, 157–166 (2002). https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1014578218041

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1014578218041

Navigation