Skip to main content
Log in

Unnatural Language Processing

  • Published:
Journal of Logic, Language and Information Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Computer-based logic proofs are a form of “unnatural” language in which the process and structure of proof generation can be observed in considerable detail. We have been studying how students respond to multimodal logic teaching, and performance measures have already indicated that students' pre-existing cognitive styles have a significant impact on teaching outcome. Furthermore, a large corpus of proofs has been gathered via automatic logging of proof development. This paper applies a series of techniques, including corpus statistical methods, to the proof logs. The results indicate that students' cognitive styles influence the structure of their logical discourse, via their differing methods of handling abstract information in diagrams, and transferring information between modalities.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Barwise, J. and Etchemendy, J., 1994, Hyperproof, Stanford, CA: CSLI Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Campagnoni, F.R. and Ehrlich, K., 1989, “Information retreival using a hypertext-based help system,” ACM Transactions on Information Systems 7, 271–291.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cox, R., Stenning, K., and Oberlander, J., 1994, “Graphical effects in learning logic: Reasoning, representation and individual differences,” pp. 237–242 in Proceedings of the 16th Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society, Atlanta, GA, August, A. Ram and K. Eisett, eds., Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • di Sessa, A.A., 1979, “On 'learnable' representations of knowledge: A meaning for the computational metaphor,” pp. 239–288 in Cognitive Process Instruction, J. Lochhead and J. Clement, eds., Philadelphia, PA: The Franklin Institute Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dunning, T., 1993, “Accurate methods for the statistics of surprise and coincidence,” Computational Linguistics 19, 61–74.

    Google Scholar 

  • Duran, R., Powers, D., and Swinton, S., 1987, “Construct validity of the GRE analytical test: A resource document,” ETS Research Report 87-11, Princeton, NJ: Educational Testing Service.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harris, R.J., 1975, A Primer of Multivariate Statistics, London: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mayer, R.E. and Sims, V.K., 1994, “For whom is a picture worth a thousand words? Extensions of a dual-coding theory of multimedia learning,” Journal of Educational Psychology 86, 389–401.

    Google Scholar 

  • Monaghan, P., 1995, “A corpus-based analysis of individual differences in proof-style,” M.Sc. Thesis, Centre for Cognitive Science, University of Edinburgh.

  • Oberlander, J., Cox, R., and Stenning, K., 1996, “Proof styles in multimodal reasoning,” pp. 403–414 in Language, Logic and Computation: Volume 1, J. Seligman and D. Westerståhl, eds., Stanford, CA: CSLI Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Oberlander, J., Cox, R., Mongahan, P., Stenning, K., and Tobin, R., 1996, “Individual differences in proof structures following multimodal logic teaching,” pp. 201–206 in Proceedings of the 18th Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society, La Jolla, CA, July, G.W. Cottrell, ed., Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schwarz, B. and Dreyfus, T., 1993, “Measuring integration of information in multirepresentational software,” Interactive Learning Environments 3, 177–198.

    Google Scholar 

  • Snow, R.E., 1987, “Aptitude complexes,” pp. 1–9 in Aptitude, Learning, and Instruction, Volume 3: Conative and Affective Process Analysis, R.E. Snow and M.J. Farr, eds., Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stenning, K. and Oberlander, J., 1991, “Reasoning with words, pictures and calculi: Computation versus justification,” pp. 607–621 in Situation Theory and Its Applications, Volume 2, J. Barwise, J.M. Gawron, G. Plotkin, and S. Tutiya, eds., Chicago, IL: Chicago University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stenning, K. and Oberlander, J., 1995, “A cognitive theory of graphical and linguistic reasoning: Logic and implementation,” Cognitive Science 19, 97–140.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stenning, K., Cox, R., and Oberlander, J., 1995, “Contrasting the cognitive effects of graphical and sentential logic teaching: Reasoning, representation and individual differences,” Language and Cognitive Processes 10, 333–354.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wason, P.C., 1977, “Self-contradictions,” pp. 114–128 in Thinking: Readings in Cognitive Science, P.N. Johnson-Laird and P.C. Wason, eds., Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Oberlander, J., Monaghan, P., Cox, R. et al. Unnatural Language Processing. Journal of Logic, Language and Information 8, 363–384 (1999). https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1008388812468

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1008388812468

Navigation