Skip to main content
Log in

The Effect of Implicit versus Explicit Comparisons on Temporal Pricing Claims

  • Published:
Marketing Letters Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Existing research has investigated the "pennies-a-day" strategy of reframing an "aggregate" expense as a "per day" expense (Nagle & Holden, 1995; Price 1995; Gourville 1998). This paper extends this research by considering the incremental impact on compliance of explicitly comparing the cost of a transaction to a specific petty cash expense (e.g., a cup of coffee). We show that in the presence of a ‘per day’ framing of price (e.g., $1 per day), an explicit comparison provides little added value. However, we also show that in the presence of an ‘aggregate’ framing of price (e.g., $350), an explicit comparison to a petty cash expense is sufficient to generate a "pennies-a-day" perspective. We conclude that it is not the ‘per day’ framing, per se, which drives "pennies-a-day" effectiveness, but the petty cash comparisons that such a framing either implicitly or explicitly generates.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Dodds, William B., Kent B. Monroe and Dhruv Grewal. (1991). “Effects of Price, Brand, and Store Information on Buyers' Product Evaluations,” Journal of Marketing Research 28 (August), 307–319.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gourville, John T. (1998). “Pennies-a-Day: The Effect of Temporal Reframing on Transaction Evaluation,” Journal of Consumer Research 24 (March), 395–408.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grewal, Dhruv, Kent B. Monroe and R. Krishnan. (1998). “The Effects of Price Comparison Advertising on Buyer's Perceptions of Acquisition Value, Transaction Value, and Behavioral Intentions,” Journal of Marketing 62(2), 46–59.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman, Daniel and Dale T. Miller. (1986). “Norm Theory: Comparing Reality to its Alternatives,” Psychological Review 93, 136–153.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, Michael D. (1984). “Consumer Choice Strategies for Comparing Noncomparable Alternatives,” Journal of Consumer Research 11 (December), 741–753.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nagle, Thomas T. and Reed K. Holden. (1995). The Strategy and Tactics of Pricing, 2nd Ed., Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Payne, John W., James R. Bettman and Eric J. Johnson. (1993). The Adaptive Decision Maker, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Price, Paul C. (1994). “Installment Framing: The Mental Aggregation and Disaggregation of Monetary Cost Over Time,” poster presented at the Annual Meeting of the Society for Judgment and Decision Making, St. Louis, MO, November 13, 1994.

  • Rosch, Eleanor and Carolyn Mervis. (1975). “Family Resemblences: Studies in the Internal Structure of Categories,” Cognitive Psychology 7 (October), 573–605.

  • Schwarz, Norbert and Herbert Bless. (1992). “Constructing Reality and Its Alternatives: An Inclusion/Exclusion Model of Assimilation and Contrast Effects in Social Judgment,” in Leonard L. Martin and Abraham Tesser (eds.) The Construction of Social Judgments; Hillsdale, New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thaler, Richard H. (1985). “Mental Accounting and Consumer Choice,” Marketing Science 4(3), 199–214.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tversky, Amos. (1977). “Features of Similarity,” Psychological Review 84(4), 327–352.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Gourville, J.T. The Effect of Implicit versus Explicit Comparisons on Temporal Pricing Claims. Marketing Letters 10, 113–124 (1999). https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1008084712717

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1008084712717

Navigation