Skip to main content
Log in

Spelling in adults: The role of reading skills and experience

Reading and Writing Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Cite this article

Abstract

One hundred university students completed tests of spellingproduction, vocabulary, reading comprehension, readingexperience, and reading accuracy (ability to distinguish apreviously read word from a similar distractor). Readingexperience, as measured by an adaptation of the AuthorRecognition Test, and reading accuracy contributed to theprediction of spelling beyond the joint contribution of readingcomprehension and vocabulary. The results are more consistentwith a uni-process model of spelling based on the quality ofword-specific orthographic learning, rather than with a dual-process account relying on both word-specific knowledge andrules.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Reference

  • Allport, D.A. & Funnell, E. (1981). Components of the mental lexicon, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B295: 397–410.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baddeley, A., Emslie, H. & Nimmo Smith, I. (1993). The Spot-the-Word text: A robust estim-ate of verbal intelligence based on lexical decision, British Journal of Clinical Psychology 32: 55–65.

    Google Scholar 

  • Behrmann, M. & Byng, S. (1992). A cognitive approach to the neurorehabilitation of acquired language disorders. In: D.I. Margolin (ed.), Cognitive neuropsychology in clinical practice (pp. 327–350). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown, A.S. (1988). Encountering misspelling and spelling performance: Why wrong isn't right, Journal of Educational Psychology 80: 488–494.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown, J., Bennett, J. & Hanna, G. (1981). The Nelson-Denny Reading Test. Lombard, IL: Riverside.

  • Burt, J.S. & Butterworth, P. (1996). Spelling in adults: Orthographic transparency, learning new letter strings, and reading accuracy, European Journal of Cognitive Psychology 8: 3–43.

    Google Scholar 

  • Burt, J.S., Walker, M.B., Humphreys, M.S. & Tehan, G. (1993). Associative priming in percep-tual identification: Effects of prime-processing requirements, Memory and Cognition 21: 125–137.

    Google Scholar 

  • Campbell, R. (1987). One or two lexicons for reading and writing words: Can misspelling shed any light?, Cognitive Neuropsychology 4: 487–499.

    Google Scholar 

  • Drake, D.A. & Ehri, L.C. (1984). Spelling acquisition: Effects of pronouncing words on memory for their spellings, Cognition and Instruction 1: 297–320.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ehri, L.C. (1986). Sources of difficulty in learning to spell and read. In: M.L. Wolrach & D. Routh (eds.), Advances in developmental and behavioral pediatrics (pp. 121–195). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ehri, L.C. (1987). Learning to read and spell words, Journal of Reading Behavior 19: 5–31.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fischer, F.W., Shankweiler, D. & Liberman, I.Y. (1985). Spelling proficiency and sensitivity to word structure, Journal of Memory and Language 24: 423–441.

    Google Scholar 

  • Foorman, B.R., Francis, D.J., Fletcher, J.M. & Lynn, A. (1996). Relation of phonological and orthographic processing to early reading: Comparing two approaches to regression-based, reading-level match designs, Journal of Educational Psychology 88: 639–652.

    Google Scholar 

  • Friedman, R.B. & Hadley, J.A. (1992). Letter-by-letter surface alexia, Cognitive Neuro-psychology 9: 185–208.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frith, U. (1980). Unexpected spelling problems. In: U. Frith (ed.), Cognitive processes in spelling (pp. 495–515). London, UK: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Henderson, L. & Chard, J. (1980). The reader's implicit knowledge of orthographic structure. In: U. Frith (ed.), Cognitive processes in spelling (pp. 85–116). London, UK: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Holmes, V.M. & Ng, E. (1993). Word-specific knowledge, word-recognition strategies, and spelling ability, Journal of Memory and Language 32: 230–257.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jacoby, L.L. & Hollingshead, A. (1990). Reading student essays may be hazardous to your spelling: Effects of reading incorrectly and correctly spelled words, Canadian Journal of Psychology 44: 345–358.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jastak, S. & Wilkinson, G.S. (1984). Wide range achievement test (revised). Wilmington, DE: Jastak Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kreiner, D.S. (1992). Reaction time measures of spelling: Testing a two-strategy model of skilled spelling, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 18: 765–776.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kreiner, D.S. & Gough, P.B. (1990). Two ideas about spelling: Rules and word-specific memory, Journal of Memory and Language 29: 103–118.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kucera, H. & Francis, W.N. (1967). Computational analysis of present-day American English. Providence, RI: Brown University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ley, R. & Long, K. (1988). Distractor similarity effects in tests of discrimination recognition and distractor-free recognition, Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 26: 407–409.

    Google Scholar 

  • Morris, D., Nelson, L. & Perney, J. (1986). Exploring the concept of "spelling instructional level" through the analysis of error types, Elementary School Journal 87: 181–200.

    Google Scholar 

  • Morton, J. (1980). The logogen model and orthographic structure. In: U. Frith (ed.), Cognitive processes in spelling (pp. 117–133). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nagy, W.E., Herman, P.A. & Anderson, R.C. (1985). Learning words from context, Reading Research Quarterly 20: 233–253.

    Google Scholar 

  • Patterson, K. (1994). Reading, writing and rehabilitation: a reckoning. In: M.J. Riddoch & G.W. Humphreys (eds.), Cognitive neuropsychology and cognitive rehabilitation (pp. 425–447). Hove, UK: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pennington, B.F. (ed.) (1991). Reading disabilities: genetic and neurological influences. Dordrecht/Boston/London: Kluwer Academic Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perfetti, C.A. (1992). The representation problem in reading acquisition. In: P. Gough, L. Ehri & R. Treiman (eds.), Reading acquisition (pp. 145–174). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shankweiler, D., Lundquist, E., Dreyer, L.G. & Dickinson, C.C. (1996). Reading and spelling difficulties in high school students: Causes and consequences, Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal 8: 267–294.

    Google Scholar 

  • Simon, D.P. & Simon, H.A. (1973). Alternative uses of phonemic information in spelling, Review of Educational Research 43: 115–137.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich, K.E. (1982). Individual differences in the cognitive processes of Reading, I: Word decoding, Journal of Learning Disabilities 15: 485–493.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich, K.E. & Cunningham, A.E. (1992). Studying the consequences of literacy within a literate society: The cognitive correlates of print exposure, Memory and Cognition 20: 51–68.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich, K.E. & Siegel, L. (1994). Phenotypic performance profile of children with reading difficulties: a regression based test of the phonological-core variable-difference model, Journal of Educational Psychology 86: 24–53.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich, K.E. & West, R.F. (1989). Exposure to print and orthographic processing, Reading Research Quarterly 21: 402–433.

    Google Scholar 

  • Templeton, S. (1991). Teaching and learning the English spelling system: Reconceptualizing method and purpose, The Elementary School Journal 92: 185.

    Google Scholar 

  • Templeton, S. & Scarborough-Franks, L. (1985). The spelling's the thing: Knowledge of derivational morphology in orthography and phonology among older students, Applied Psycholinguistics 6: 371–389.

    Google Scholar 

  • Turner, M.L. & Pinkston, R.S. (1993). Effects of a memory and aging workshop on negative beliefs of memory loss in the elderly, Educational Gerontology 19: 359–373.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tyler, A. & Nagy, W. (1989). The acquisition of English derivational morphology, Journal of Memory and Language 28: 649–667.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weekes, B.S. (1994). Spelling skills of lexical readers, British Journal of Psychology 85: 245–257.

    Google Scholar 

  • Whissell, C. & Kienapple, C. (1995). The false recognition of mood-matched and mood-opposite words in a memory task: introduction of a delayed testing condition, Perceptual and Motor Skills 81: 659–664.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and Permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Burt, J.S., Fury, M.B. Spelling in adults: The role of reading skills and experience. Reading and Writing 13, 1–30 (2000). https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1008071802996

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1008071802996

Navigation