Skip to main content
Log in

Package Graphics and Consumer Product Beliefs

  • Published:
Journal of Business and Psychology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

In recent years, marketers' tactics to increase product sales through health claims have been scrutinized by public policy makers. The culmination of this process was the passage of the National Labeling Education Act, which highly restricts the type, amount, and format of nutritional information conveyed on the package. In this investigation, we examine an under-explored aspect of the package—its colors and pictures—to determine what, if any, impact these aspects have on consumer beliefs regarding important product characteristics. We find that even when very concrete verbal information is used, graphical representations have a significant and long-term effect on product beliefs and purchase intentions.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Similar content being viewed by others

REFERENCES

  • Anderson, N. H. (1981). Integration theory applied to cognitive responses and attitudes. In R. E. Petty, T. M. Ostrom, & T. C. Brock (Eds.), Cognitive Responses in Persuasion (pp. 361–397). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Andrews, J. C., Akhter, S. H., Durvasula, S., & Muehling, D. D. (1992). Effects of advertising distinctiveness and message content involvement on cognitive and affective responses to advertising. Current Issues and Research in Advertising, 14 (Spring), 45–58.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bone, P. F., Shimp, T. A., & Sharma, S. (1990). Assimilation and contrast effects in product performance perceptions: implications for public policy. Journal of Public Policy and Marketing, 9, 100–110.

    Google Scholar 

  • Commerce Clearing House Trade Regulation Reports (1988). Deception defined (10828–10835).

  • Crystal clear persuasion. (1993, January). Tufts University Diet and Nutrition Letter, p. 1.

  • Deighton, J. (1984). The interaction of advertising and evidence. Journal of Consumer Research, 11 (December), 763–770.

  • Deighton, J., & Schindler, R. M. (1988). Can advertising influence experience? Psychology and Marketing, 5 (Summer), 103–115.

    Google Scholar 

  • Edell, J. A., & Staelin, R. (1983). The information processing of print advertisements. Journal of Consumer Research, 10 (June), 45–61.

  • Ellen, P. S., & Bone, P. F. (1990). Measuring communication evoked imagery processing. In R. H. Holman & M. R. Solomon (Eds.), Advances in Consumer Research, 18 (pp. 806–812). Provo, UT: Association for Consumer Research.

    Google Scholar 

  • Federal Register (1990). Food labeling, reference daily intakes and daily reference values (55, Mo. 139, p. 29476–29486).

    Google Scholar 

  • Federal Trade Commission Deception Policy Statement (1983). Letter from James C. Miller, III (Former) Chairman, Federal Trade Commission, to the Honorable John D. Dingell, Chairman, Committee on Energy and Commerce, U.S. House of Representatives. (October 14, 1–19).

  • Food and Drug Administration (1994). Guide to nutritional labeling and education act (NLEA) requirements (August).

  • Hoch, S. J., & Ha, Y. W. (1986). Consumer learning: advertising and the ambiguity of product experience. Journal of Consumer Research, 13 (September), 221–233.

  • Houston, M. J., Childers, T. L., & Heckler, S. E. (1987). Picture-word consistency and the elaborative processing of advertisements.Journal of Marketing Research, 25 (November), 359–369.

  • Jacobs, D. L. (1994, May 29). The titans of tint make their picks. New York Times, p. F7.

  • Johar, G. V. (1995). Consumer involvement and deception from implied advertising claims. Journal of Marking Research, 32 (August), 267–279.

  • Kisielius, J., & Sternthal, B. (1984). Detecting and explaining vividness effects in attitudinal judgments. Journal of Marketing Research, 21 (February), 54–64.

  • Kisielius, J., & Sternthal, B. (1986). Examining the vividness controversy: an availabilityvalence interpretation. Journal of Consumer Research, 12 (March), 418–431.

  • Kober Hats, Inc. v. FTC (CA-1;1962) 1963 TRADE CASES 70,597,311 F. 2d 358 (FTC Dkt. 8190).

  • Laczniak, R., & Grossbart, S. (1990). An assessment of assumptions underlying the reasonable consumer element in deceptive advertising policy. Journal of Public Policy & Marketing, 9, 85–99.

    Google Scholar 

  • Madden, T. J., Allen, C. T., & Twible, J. (1988). Attitude toward the ad: an assessment of diverse measurement indices under different processing 'sets.' Journal Marketing Research, 25 (August), 242–252.

  • Meyers-Levy, J., & Peracchio, L. A. (1995). Understanding the effects of color: how the correspondence between available and required resources affects attitude. Journal of Consumer Research, 22 (September), 121–138.

  • Miniard, P. W., Bhatla, S., Lord, K. R., Dickson, P. R., & Unnava, H. R. (1991). Picture based persuasion processes and the moderating role of involvement. Journal of Consumer Research, 18(June), 92–107.

  • Nabisco Public Relations Department, Personal correspondence, September 29, 1998.

  • Nisbett, R., & Ross, L. (1980). Human inference: strategies and shortcomings of social judgment. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall, Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Petty, R. E., & Cacioppo, J. C. (1986). Communication and persuasion: central and peripheral routes to attitude change. New York: Springer-Verlag.

    Google Scholar 

  • Petty, R. E., Cacioppo, J. C., & Schumann, D. (1983). Central and peripheral routes to advertising effectiveness: the moderating role of involvement. Journal of Consumer Research, 10 (September), 135–146.

  • Pylyshyn, Z. W. (1973). What the mind's eye tells the mind's brain: a critique of mental imagery. Psychological Bulletin, 80, 1–24.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ramirez, A. (1990, July 5). Lessons in the cracker market: Nabisco saved new graham snack. New York Times, p. D1.

  • Reyes, R. M., Thompson, W. L., & Bower, G. H. (1980). Judgmental biases resulting from differing availabilities of arguments. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39, 2–12.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shimp, T. A., Hyatt, E. M., & Snyder, D. J. (1991). A critical appraisal of demand artifacts in consumer research.Journal of Consumer Research, 18(December), 273–283.

  • Simon, H. A. (1967). An information-processing explanation of some perceptual phenomena. British Journal of Psychology, 58, 1–12.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sullivan, A. M., Andrews, E. A., Hollinghurst, F., Maddigan, R., & Noseworthy, R. M. (1976–77). The relative effectiveness of instructional television. Interchange, 7, 46–51.

    Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, S. E., & Thompson, S. C. (1982). Stalking the elusive ‘vividness’ effect. Psychological Review, 89 (2), 155–181.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Bone, P.F., France, K.R. Package Graphics and Consumer Product Beliefs. Journal of Business and Psychology 15, 467–489 (2001). https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1007826818206

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1007826818206

Navigation