Advertisement

Environmental Biology of Fishes

, Volume 51, Issue 4, pp 421–428 | Cite as

Acoustic behavior of the damselfish Dascyllus albisella: behavioral and geographic variation

  • David A. Mann
  • Phillip S. Lobel
Article

Abstract

Behavioral and geographic variation in animal communication has been well-studied in insects, frogs, birds, and mammals, but little is known about variation in fishes. We used underwater audio-video recordings of the behavior and associated sounds produced by the domino damselfish, Dascyllus albisella, at Johnston Atoll and Hawaii, which are separated by 1000 km, to study behavioral and geographic variation in communication sounds. Males produced pulsed sounds during the courtship behavior known as the signal jump, visiting by females (during pseudospawning), mating, aggression to heterospecifics and conspecifics, and nest preparation. Females made only aggressive sounds. The following features of the sounds were measured: number of pulses, pulse rate, pulse duration, inter-pulse interval, dominant frequency, and frequency envelope. The only difference between visiting and mating sounds was a small difference in pulse duration. Two types of aggressive sounds were produced, pops and chirps. Pops contained only one or two pulses and were more commonly made towards heterospecifics than conspecifics. Aggressive chirps had between 3–11 pulses and were made most often towards conspecifics. The pulse rate of aggressive chirps was faster than signal jump sounds. The only difference in signal jump sounds made by males from Johnston Atoll and Hawaii, was a small difference in pulse duration, which was likely due to differences in the depths of the recording environment and not in the sounds produced.

sound production courtship aggression spawning Johnston Atoll Hawaii 

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References cited

  1. Amorim, M.C.P.D. 1996. Sound production in the blue-green damselfish, Chromis viridis (Cuvier, 1830) (Pomacentridae). Bioacoustics 6: 265–272.Google Scholar
  2. Chen, K.-C. & H.-K. Mok. 1988. Sound production in the anemonefishes, Amphiprion clarkii and A. frenatus (Pomacentridae), in captivity. Japan. J. Ichthyol. 35: 90–97.Google Scholar
  3. Claridge, M.F. & J.C. Morgan. 1993. Geographical variation in the acoustic signal of the planthopper, Nilaparvata bakeri (Muir), in Asia: species recognition and sexual selection. Biol. J. Linn. Soc. 48: 267–281.Google Scholar
  4. Ewing. 1989. Arthropod bioacoustics: neurobiology and behaviour. Cornell University Press, Ithaca. 260 pp.Google Scholar
  5. Fay, R.R. & S. Coombs. 1983. Neural mechanisms in sound detection and temporal summation. Hear. Res. 10: 69–92.Google Scholar
  6. Fine, M.L. 1978. Seasonal and geographical variation of the mating call of the oyster toadfish Opsanus tau L. Oecologia 36: 45–57.Google Scholar
  7. Gerald, J.W. 1971. Sound production during courtship in six species of sunfish (Centrarchidae). Evolution 25: 75–87.Google Scholar
  8. Godwin, J. 1995. Phylogenetic and habitat influences on mating system structure in the humbug damselfishes (Dascyllus, Pomacentridae). Bull. Mar. Sci. 57: 637–652.Google Scholar
  9. Grant, B.R. & P.R. Grant. 1989. Evolutionary dynamics of a natural population. Chicago University Press, Chicago. 350 pp.Google Scholar
  10. Ha, S.J. 1973. Aspects of sound communication in the damselfish Eupomacentrus partitus. Ph.D. Thesis, University of Miami, Miami. 78 pp.Google Scholar
  11. Holzberg, S. 1973. Beobachtungen zur Ökologie und zum socialverhalten des Korallenbarsches Dascyllus marginatus Ruppell (Pisces; Pomacentridae). Z. Tierpsychol. 33: 492–513.Google Scholar
  12. Kenyon, T.N. 1994. The significance of sound interception to males of the bicolor damselfish, Pomacentrus partitus, during courtship. Env. Biol. Fish. 40: 391–405.Google Scholar
  13. Lobel, P.S. 1996. Spawning sound of the trunkfish, Ostracion meleagris (Ostraciidae). Biol. Bull. 191: 308–309.Google Scholar
  14. Lobel, P.S. & D.A. Mann. 1995. Spawning sounds of the domino damselfish, Dascyllus albisella (Pomacentridae), and the relationship to male size. Bioacoustics 6: 187–198.Google Scholar
  15. Luh, H.K. & H.K. Mok. 1986. Sound production in the domino damselfish Dascyllus trimaculatus (Pomacentridae) under laboratory conditions. Japan. J. Icthyol. 33: 70–74.Google Scholar
  16. Mann, D.A. & P.S. Lobel. 1995. Passive acoustic detection of sounds produced by the damselfish, Dascyllus albisella (Pomacentridae). Bioacoustics 6: 199–213.Google Scholar
  17. Mann, D.A. & P.S. Lobel. 1997. Propagation of damselfish (Pomacentridae) courtship sounds. J. Acoust. Soc. Amer. 101: 3783–3791.Google Scholar
  18. Morton, E. 1977. On the occurrence and significance of motivation-structural rules in some bird and mammal species. Amer. Nat. 111: 855–869.Google Scholar
  19. Morton, E. & J. Page. 1992. Animal talk. Random House, New York. 275 pp.Google Scholar
  20. Myrberg, A.A., Jr. 1972. Ethology of the bicolor damselfish, Eupomacentrus partitus (Pisces: Pomacentridae): a comparative analysis of laboratory and field behavior. Anim. Behav. Mon. 5: 197–283.Google Scholar
  21. Myrberg, A.A., Jr., E. Kramer & P. Heinecke. 1965. Sound production by cichlid fishes. Science 149: 555–558.Google Scholar
  22. Myrberg, A.A., Jr., M. Mohler & J.D. Catala. 1986. Sound production by males of a coral reef fish (Pomacentrus partitus): its significance to females. Anim. Behav. 34: 913–923.Google Scholar
  23. Myrberg, A.A., Jr., Ha, S.J. & M.J. Shamblott. 1993. The sounds of bicolor damselfish (Pomacentrus partitus): predictors of body size and a spectral basis for individual recognition and assessment. J. Acoust. Soc. Amer. 94: 3067–3070.Google Scholar
  24. Nelissen, M.H.J. 1977. Sound production by Haplochromis burtoni (Gunther) and Tropheus boulenger (Pisces, Cichlidae). Annales Soc. R. Zool. Belg. 106: 155–166.Google Scholar
  25. Nelissen, M.H.J. 1978. Sound production by some Tanganyikan cichlid fishes and a hypothesis for the evolution of their communication mechanisms. Behav. 64: 137–147.Google Scholar
  26. Randall, H.A. & G.R. Allen. 1977. A revision of the damselfish genus Dascyllus (Pomacentridae) with the description of a new species. Records of the Australian Museum 31: 349–385.Google Scholar
  27. Randall, J.E., P.S. Lobel & E.H. Chave. 1985. Annotated checklist of the fishes of Johnston Island. Pac. Sci. 39: 24–80.Google Scholar
  28. Ryan, M.J. & A.S. Rand. 1993. Species recognition and sexual selection as a unitary problem in animal communication. Evolution 47: 647–657.Google Scholar
  29. Spanier, E. 1970. Analysis of sounds and associated behavior of the domino damselfish Dascyllus trimaculatus (Ruppell, 1828) (Pomacentridae). M.Sc. Thesis, Tel-Aviv University, Tel-Aviv. 80 pp.Google Scholar
  30. Spanier, E. 1979. Aspects of species recognition by sound in four species of damselfishes, genus Eupomacentrus (Pisces: Pomacentridae). Z. Tierpsychol. 51: 301–316.Google Scholar
  31. Wells, M.M. & C.S. Henry. 1992. The role of courtship songs in reproductive isolation among populations of green lacewings of the genus Chrysoperla (Neuroptera: Chrysopidae). Evolution 46: 31–42.Google Scholar
  32. Wilczynski, W., A. C. Keddy-Hector & M.J. Ryan. 1992. Call patterns and basilar papilla tuning in cricket frogs. I. Differences among populations and between sexes. Brain Behav. Evol. 39: 229–237.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Kluwer Academic Publishers 1998

Authors and Affiliations

  • David A. Mann
    • 1
  • Phillip S. Lobel
    • 2
  1. 1.Woods Hole Oceanographic InstitutionWoods HoleU.S.A
  2. 2.Boston University Marine ProgramMarine Biological LaboratoryWoods HoleU.S.A

Personalised recommendations