Skip to main content
Log in

Anchoring Induced Biases in Consumer Price Negotiations

  • Published:
Journal of Consumer Policy Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Two studies were conducted with the aim of demonstrating anchoring induced biases in consumer price negotiations.

In Study 1, 96 undergraduate students of business administration who were recruited as subjects played the role of buyers of a condominium. All subjects were given the same market information. They were then asked to state whether their reservation price was higher or lower than an arbitrary price example (irrelevant anchor) that for different groups of subjects was either low or high. Finally, subjects indicated their reservation price. As would be predicted if adjustments from the anchor are insufficient, the indicated reservation price was lower when the anchor was low than when it was high.

In Study 2, employing 64 undergraduate students of psychology who conducted dyad negotiations about the price of condominiums, the effect of the irrelevant anchor on the initially indicated reservation price was replicated. In addition, an anchoring effect of the seller's initial offer was observed. The results also revealed effects of both irrelevant anchor and initial offer on the purchase price.

From a public policy point of view, the results imply that consumers may be strongly influenced by irrelevant anchors provided by sellers. Provision of accurate market price information may however lessen the impact of irrelevant anchors.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

REFERENCES

  • Blount, S., Thomas-Hunt, M. C., & Neale, M. A. (1996). The price is right-or is it? A reference point model of two-party negotiations. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 68, 1–12.

    Google Scholar 

  • Camerer, C. F. (1989). An experimental test of several generalized utility theories. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty, 2, 61–104.

    Google Scholar 

  • Camerer, C. F., & Hogarth, R. M. (1999). The effects of financial incentives in experiments: A review and capital-labor-production framework. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty, 19, 7–42.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chapman, G. B., & Johnson, E. J. (1994). The limits of anchoring. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 7, 223–242.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gärling, T., & Friman, M. (in press). A psychological conceptualization of residential choice and satisfaction. In: J. Aragonés, G. Francescato, & T. Gärling (Eds.), Residential environments: Choice, satisfaction, and behavior. Westport, CT: Greenwood.

  • Güth, W. P., Schmittberger, P., & Schwarze, B. (1982). An experimental analysis of ultimatum bargaining. Journal of Economic Behavior and Organizations, 3, 367–388.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hogarth, R. M., & Einhorn, H. J. (1990). Venture theory: A model of decision weights. Management Science, 36, 780–803.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jacowitz, K. E., & Kahneman, D. (1995). Measures of anchoring in estimation tasks. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 21, 1161–1166.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman, D. (1992). Reference points, anchors, norms, and mixed feelings. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 51, 296–312.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1979). Prospect theory: An analysis of decision under risk. Econometrica, 47, 263–291.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kristensen, H., & Gärling, T. (1997a). Adoption of cognitive reference point in negotiations. Acta Psychologica, 97, 277–288.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kristensen, H., & Gärling, T. (1997b). Determinants of buyers' aspiration and reservation price. Journal of Economic Psychology, 18, 487–503.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kristensen, H., & Gärling, T. (1997c). The effects of anchor points and reference points on negotiation process and outcome. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 71, 85–94.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kristensen, H., & Gärling, T. (2000). Anchor points, reference points, and counteroffers in negotiations. Group Decision and Negotiation, 9, 493–505.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kühberger, A. (1998). The influence of framing on risky decisions: A meta-analysis. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 75, 23–55.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lindberg, E., Gärling, T., & Montgomery, H. (1989). Belief-value structures as determinants of consumer behavior: A study of housing preferences and choices. Journal of Consumer Policy, 12, 119–137.

    Google Scholar 

  • Neale, M. A., & Bazerman, M. H. (1991). Cognition and rationality in negotiation. New York: Free Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Neale, M. A., & Northcraft, G. B. (1991). Behavioral negotiation theory: A framework for conceptualizing dyadic bargaining. In L. J. Cummings & B. M. Staw (Eds.), Research in organizational behavior, Vol. 13, pp. 147–190. Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Northcraft, G. B., & Neale, M. A. (1987). Expert, amateurs, and real estate: An anchoring-and-adjustment perpective on property pricing decisions. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 39, 228–241.

    Google Scholar 

  • Payne, J. W., Bettman, J. R., & Johnson, E. J. (1993). The adaptive decision maker. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pinkley, R. L., Neale, M. A., & Bennett, R. J. (1994). The impact of alternatives to settlement in dyadic negotiation. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 57, 97–116.

    Google Scholar 

  • Raiffa, H. (1982). The art and science of negotiation. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ritov, I. (1996). Anchoring in simulated competitive market negotiation. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 67, 16–25.

    Google Scholar 

  • Simon, H. A. (1990). Invariants of human behavior. Annual Review of Psychology, 41, 1–19.

    Google Scholar 

  • Slovic, P., & Lichtenstein, S. (1971). Comparison of Bayesian and regression approaches to the study of information processing in judgement. Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 6, 649–744.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thompson, L. (1990). Negotiation behavior and outcomes: Empirical evidence and theoretical issues. Psychological Bulletin, 108, 515–532.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1974). Judgement under uncertainty: Heuristics and biases. Science, 185, 1124–1131.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1991). Loss aversion in riskless choice: A referencedependent model. Quarterly Journal of Economics, 106, 1039–1061.

    Google Scholar 

  • Walton, R. E., & McKersie, R. B. (1965). A behavioral theory of labor relations. New York: McGraw-Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • White, S. B., & Neale, M. A. (1991). Reservation price, resistance point, and BATNAs: Determine the parameters of acceptable negotiated outcomes. Negotiation Journal, 7, 379–388.

    Google Scholar 

  • White, S. B., Valley, K. L., Bazerman, M. H., Neale, M. A., & Peck, S. R. (1994). Alternative models of price behavior in dyadic negotiations: Market prices, reservation prices, and negotiator aspirations. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 57, 430–447.

    Google Scholar 

  • Whyte, G., & Sebenius, J. K. (1997). The effect of multiple anchors on anchoring in individual and group judgement. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 69, 75–85.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Kristensen, H., Gärling, T. Anchoring Induced Biases in Consumer Price Negotiations. Journal of Consumer Policy 23, 445–460 (2000). https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1007280722313

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1007280722313

Keywords

Navigation