Skip to main content
Log in

Gender Schema and Social Judgments: A Developmental Study of Children from Hong Kong

  • Published:
Sex Roles Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The purpose of the study was to examine inferences and judgments of gender schematic and aschematic children. Kindergarten, third grade, and sixth grade Hong Kong Chinese children (n = 196) were categorized into schematic and aschematic groups on the basis of their reaction times to gender-stereotypic and counterstereotypic items. The children were then asked to make several inferences and judgments about four male and female targets behaving gender-stereotypically or counterstereotypically. Both age and gender schematicity were related to the children's inferences. Older children and gender-schematic children relied more on individuating information and younger children relied more on the gender label. The results are discussed within the framework of gender-schema theories.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

REFERENCES

  • Ames, P. T., Dissanayake, W., & Kasulis, T. P. (1994). Self as person in Asian theory and practice. Albany, NY: State University of New York Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Archer, J. (1991). A methodological commentary on gender schema research. British Journal of Social Psychology, 30, 185–188.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bargh, J. A., Bond, R. N., Lombardi, W. J., & Tota, M. E. (1986). The additive nature of chronic and temporary sources of construct accessibility. Journal of Personalty and Social Psychology, 50, 869–878.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bargh, J. A., & Thein, R. (1985). Individual constructs accessibility, person memory, and recall- judgement link: the case of information overload. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology.

  • Bem, S. L. (1981). Gender schema theory: A cognitive account of sex typing. Psychological Review, 88, 354–364.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berndt, T. J., & Heller, K. A. (1986). Gender stereotypes and social inferences: A developmental study. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50, 889–898.

    Google Scholar 

  • Biernat, M. (1991). Gender stereotypes and the relationship between masculinity and femininity: A developmental analysis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61, 351–365.

    Google Scholar 

  • Blakemore, J. E., LaRue, A. A., & Olejnik, A. B. (1978). Sex appropriate toy preference and the ability to conceptualize toys as sex-role related. Developmental Psychology, 15, 339–340.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bond, M. H., & King, A. Y. C. (1986). Coping with the threat of Westernization in Hong Kong. International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 9, 351–364.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carter, D. B., & Levy, G. D. (1988). Cognitive aspects of early sex-role development: The influence of gender schemas on preschooler's memories and preferences for sex-typed toys and activities. Child Development, 59, 782–792.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carter, D. B., & Levy, G. D. (1991). Gender schemas and the salience of gender: Individual differences in non-reversal discrimination learning. Sex Roles, 25, 555–567.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chiu, C. (1988). The effects of personality and performance on reward allocation. Journal of Social Psychology, 128, 279–280.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chiu, C. (1989). Reward allocation among Chinese high school students in Hong Kong. Journal of Social Psychology, 129, 751–757.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chiu, C., & Hong, Y. (1992). The effects of intentionality and validation on individual and collective responsibility attribution among Hong Kong Chinese. Journal of Psychology, 126, 291–300.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, J., & Cohen, P. (1983). Applied multiple regression/correlation analysis for behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Deaux, K., Kite, M. E., & Lewis, L. L. (1985). Clustering and gender schemata: An uncertain look. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 11, 387–397.

    Google Scholar 

  • Deaux, K., & Lewis, L. L. (1984). Structure of gender stereotypes: Interrelationships among components and gender label. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 46, 991–1004.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dunlap, W. P., & Kemery, E. R. (1987). Failure to detect moderating effects: Is multicollinearity the problem? Psychological Bulletin, 102, 418–420.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fagot, B. I., & Leinbach, M. D. (1989). The young child's gender schema: Environmental input, internal organization. Child Development, 60, 663–672.

    Google Scholar 

  • Farrar, M. J., & Goodman, G. S. (1990). Developmental differences in the relations between scripts and episodic memory: Do they exist? In R. Fivush & J. Hudson (Eds.), Knowing and remembering in young children (pp. 30–64). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Graesser, A. C., Gordon, S. E., & Sawyer, J. D. (1979). Recognition memory for typical and atypical actions in scripted activities: Tests of a script pointer + tag hypothesis. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 18, 319–332.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harter, S., & Buddin, B. J. (1987). Children's understanding of the simultaneity of two emotions: A five-stage developmental acquisition sequence. Developmental Psychology, 23, 388–399.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hartup, W. W. (1989). Social relationships and their developmental significance. American Psychologist, 44, 120–126.

    Google Scholar 

  • Higgins, E. T. (1981). Role taking and social judgment: Alternative developmental perspectives and processes. In J. H. Flavell & L. Ross (Eds.), Social cognitive development: Frontiers and possible futures. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Higgins, T. E. (1996). Knowledge activation: Accessibility, applicability, and salience. In T. E. Higgins & A. W. Kruglanski (Eds.), Social psychology; Handbook of basic principles. New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Higgins, E. T., & Bargh, J. A. (1987). Social cognition and social perceptions. Annual Review of Psychology, 38, 39–425.

    Google Scholar 

  • Higgins, E. T., & King, G. (1981). Accessibility of social constucts: Information-processing consequences of individual and contexual variability. In N. Cantor & J. F. Khilstrom (Eds.), Personality, cognition and social interaction. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hsu, F. L. K. (1985). The self in cross-cultural perspective. In A. J. Marsella, G. DeVos, & F. L. K. Hsu (Eds.), Cultural and self (pp. 24–55). NewYork: Tavistock.

    Google Scholar 

  • Katz, P. S., & Boswell, S. (1986). Flexibility and traditionality in children's gender roles. Genetic Social and General Psychology Monographs, 112, 105–147.

    Google Scholar 

  • Katz, P. S., & Ksansnak, K. R. (1994). Developmental aspects of gender role flexibility and traditionality in middle childhood and adolescence. Developmental Psychology, 30, 272–282.

    Google Scholar 

  • Keyes, S. (1983). Sex differences in cognitive abilities and sex role stereotypes in Hong Kong Chinese adolescents. Sex Roles, 9(8), 853–870.

    Google Scholar 

  • Keyes, S. (1984). Measuring sex-role stereotypes: Attitudes among Hong Kong Chinese adolescents and the development of the Chinese sex-role inventory. Sex Roles, 10, 129–140.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kohlberg, L. (1966). A cognitive-developmental analysis of children's sex role concepts and attitudes. In E. E. Maccoby (Ed.), The development of sex differences. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kondo, D. (1990). Crafting selves: Power, gender, and discourses of identity in a Japanese work place. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuhn, D., Nash, S. C., & Brucken, L. (1978). Sex role concepts of two-and three-year-old children. Child Development, 49, 445–451.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lau, S. (1988). The value orientation of Chinese university students in Hong Kong. International Journal of Psychology, 23, 583–596.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lau, S., & Wong, A. K. (1992). Value and sex-role orientation of Chinese adolescents. International Journal of Psychology, 27, 3–17.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lebra, T. S. (1976). Japanese patterns of behavior. Honolulu, HI: East-West Center.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levy, G. D. (1994). High and low gender schematic children's release from proactive interference. Sex Roles, 30, 93–108.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levy, G. D., & Carter, D. (1989). Gender schema, gender constancy, and gender-role knowledge: The role of cognitive factors in preschooler's gender-role stereotype attribution. Developmental Psychology, 25, 444–449.

    Google Scholar 

  • Liben, L. S., & Signorella, M. L. (1993). Gender schematic processing in children: The role of initial interpretations of stimuli. Developmental Psychology, 29(1), 141–149.

    Google Scholar 

  • Linville, P. W., Fischer, G. W., & Salovey, P. (1989). Perceived distributions of the characteristics of in-group and out-group members: Empirical evidence and computer simulation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 165–188.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lobel, T. E. (1994). Sex typing and the social perception of gender stereotypic and nonstereotypic behavior: The uniqueness of feminine males. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66, 379–385.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lobel, T. E., Bempechat, J., Gewirtz, J., Shoken-Topaz, T., & Bach, E. (1993). The role of gender-related information and self-endorsement of traits in preadolescents inferences and judgments. Child Development, 64, 1285–1294.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maccoby, E. E. (1988). Gender as a social category. Developmental Psychology, 24, 755–765.

    Google Scholar 

  • Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. (1991). Culture and the self: Implications for cognition, emotion, and motivation. Psychological Bulletin, 98, 224–253.

    Google Scholar 

  • Martin, C. L. (1989). Children's use of gender-related information in making social judgments. Developmental Psychology, 25, 80–88.

    Google Scholar 

  • Martin, C. L. (1993). New directions for investigating children's gender knowledge. Developmental Review, 13, 184–204.

    Google Scholar 

  • Martin, C. L., & Halverson, C. F. (1981). A schematic processing model of sex typing and stereotyping in children. Child Development, 52, 1119–1132.

    Google Scholar 

  • Martin, C. L., & Halverson, C. F., Jr. (1987). The role of cognition in sex roles and sex typing. In D. B. Carter (Ed.), Current conceptions of sex roles and sex typing: Theory and research (pp. 123–137). New York: Praeger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Martin, C. L., & Little, J. K. (1990). The relation of gender understanding to children's sextyped preferences and gender stereotypes. Child Development, 61, 1427–1439.

    Google Scholar 

  • Martin, C. L., Wood, C. H., & Little, J. K. (1990). The development of gender stereotype components. Child Development, 61, 1891–1904.

    Google Scholar 

  • O'Keefe, E. S., & Hyde, J. S. (1983). The development of occupational sex-role stereotypes: The effects of gender stability and age. Sex Roles, 9, 481–492.

    Google Scholar 

  • Papalia, D. E., & Tennet, S. S. (1975). Vocational aspirations in preschoolers. Sex Roles, 1, 197–199.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rubin, K. H. (1998). Social and emotional development from a cultural perspective, Developmental Psychology, 34, 611–615.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ruble, D. N., & Martin, C. L. (1998). Gender development. In W. Damon and N. Eisenberg (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology, Vol. 3: Social, emotional and personality development (5th ed., pp. 933–1016). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ruble, D. N., & Stangor, C. (1986). Stalking the elusive schema: Insights from developmental and social-psychological analyses of gender schemas. Social Cognition, 4, 227–261.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schank, R. C., & Abelson, R. (1977). Scripts, plans, goals and understanding. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shweder, R. E. (1990). Cultural psychology—what is it? In J. W. Stigler, R. E. Shweder, & C. Herdt (Eds.), Cultural psychology: Essays on comparative human development. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Signorella, M. L. (1987). Gender schemata: Individual differences and context effects. In L. S. Liben & M. L. Signorella (Eds.), Children's gender schemata: New directions in child development (pp. 23–37). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

    Google Scholar 

  • Signorella, M. L., Bigler, R. S., & Liben, L. S. (1993). Developmental differences in children's gender schemata about others: A meta-analytic review. Developmental Review, 13(2), 147–183.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith, E. R., & Zarate, M. A. (1992). Exemplar-based model of social judgement. Psychological Review, 99, 3–21.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spence, J. T. (1984). Masculinity, femininity and gender related traits: A conceptual analysis and critique of current research. In B. A. Maher & B. Maher (Eds.), Progress in Experimental Personality Research, 13, 1–97.

  • Spence, J. T. (1993). Gender related traits and gender ideology: Evidence for a multifactorial theory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64(4), 624–635.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stangor, C., & McMillan, D. (1992). Memory for expectancy-congruent and expectancyincongruent information: A review of the social and social developmental literature. Psychological Bulletin, 111, 42–61.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sun, L. (1983). The deep structure of the Chances culture. Hong Kong: Yi Shan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thompson, S. K. (1975). Gender labels and early sex-role development. Child Development, 46, 339–347.

    Google Scholar 

  • Triandis, H. C. (1989). The self and social behavior in differing cultural contexts. Psychological Review, 96, 506–520.

    Google Scholar 

  • Turner, P. J., & Gervai, J. (1995). A multidimensional study of gender typing in preschool children and parents: Personality, attitudes, preferences, and cultural differences. Developmental Psychology, 31(5), 759–779.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yau, J., & Smetana, J. G. (1996). Adolescent- parent conflict among Chinese adolescents in Hong Kong. Child Development, 67, 1262–1265.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Lobel, T.E., Bar-David, E., Gruber, R. et al. Gender Schema and Social Judgments: A Developmental Study of Children from Hong Kong. Sex Roles 43, 19–42 (2000). https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1007035611440

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1007035611440

Keywords

Navigation