Adrien, J. L., Lenoir, P., Martineau, J., Perrot, A., Hameury, L., Larmande, C., & Sauvage, D. (1993). Blind ratings of early symptoms of autism based upon family home movies. Journal of American Academy of Child Adolescent Psychiatry, 32617-626.
Google Scholar
American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders(4th ed.). Washington, DC: Author.
Google Scholar
Baron-Cohen, S., & Swettenham, J. (1997). Theory of mind in autism: Its relationship to executive function and central coherence. In D. J. Cohen, & F. R. Volkmar (Eds.), Handbook of autism and pervasive developmental disorders(pp. 880-893). New York: Wiley.
Google Scholar
Bauminger, N., & Kasari, C. (1999). Brief report: Theory of mind in high-functioning children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 2981-86.
Google Scholar
Berg, E. A. (1948). A simple objective technique for measuring flexibility of thinking. Journal of General Psychology, 3915-22.
Google Scholar
Berger, H. J. C., van Hoof, J. J. M., van Spaendonck, K. P. M., Horstink, M. W. I. M., van den Bercken, J. H. L., Jaspers, R., & Cools, A. R. (1989). Haloperidol and cognitive shifting. Neuropsychologia, 27629-639.
Google Scholar
Berger, H. J. C., van Spaendonck, K. P. M., Horstink, M. W. I. M., Buytenhuijs, E. L., Lammers, P. W. J. M., & Cools, A. R. (1993). Cognitive shifting as a predictor of progress in social understanding in high-functioning adolescents with autism: A prospective study. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 23341-359.
Google Scholar
Berger, H. J. C., van Es, N., Teunisse, J. P., Horstink, M. W. I. M., van Spaendonck, K. P. M., & Cools, A. R. (1999). Relationship between memory strategies and motor symptoms in Parkinson's disease. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 21676-683.
Google Scholar
Brian, J. A., & Bryson, S. E. (1996). Disembedding performance and recognition memory in autism/PDD. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 37865-872.
Google Scholar
Buytenhuijs, E. L., Berger, H. J. C., van Spaendonck, K. P. M., Horstink, M. W. I. M., Borm, G. F., & Cools, A. R. (1994). Memory and learning strategies in patients with Parkinson's disease. Neuropsychologia, 32335-342.
Google Scholar
Ciesielski, K. T., & Harris, R. J. (1997). Factors relating to performance failure on executive tasks in autism. Child Neuropsychology, 31-12.
Google Scholar
Cools, A. R., Berger, H. J. C., Buytenhuijs, E. L., Horstink, M. W. I. M., & van Spaendonck, K. P. M. (1993). Manifestations of switching disorders in animals and man with dopamine deficits in A10 and/or A9 circuitries. In E. C. Wolters & P. Scheltens (Eds.), Mental dysfunction in Parkinson's disease (pp. 49-67). Amsterdam: Vrije Universiteit.
Google Scholar
Delis, D. C., Kramer, J. H., Kaplan, E., & Ober, B. A. (1987). The California Verbal Learning Test: Research edition.New York: Psychological Corp.
Google Scholar
Downes, J. J., Roberts, A. C., Sahakian, B. J., Evenden, J. L., Morris, R. G., & Robbins, T. W. (1989). Impaired extra-dimensional shift performance in medicated and unmedicated Parkinson's disease: evidence for a specific attentional dysfunction. Neuropsychologia, 271329-1343.
Google Scholar
Ekstrom, R. B., French, J. W., & Harman, H. H. (1976). Manual for kit of factor-referenced cognitive tests.Princeton, NJ: Educational Testing Service.
Google Scholar
Evers, A., van Vliet-Mulder, J. C., & ter Laak, J. (1992). Documentatie van tests en testresearch in Nederland.Assen: Van Gorcum.
Google Scholar
Frith, U. (1989). Autism: Explaining the enigma.Oxford, UK: Blackwell.
Google Scholar
Frith, U., & Happé, F. (1994). Autism: Beyond “theory of mind”. Cognition, 50115-132.
Google Scholar
Fridlund, A. J., & Delis, D. C. (1987). The California Verbal Learning Test, scoring and administration software.New York: Psychological Corp.
Google Scholar
Grant, D. A., & Berg, E. A. (1948). A behavioral analysis of degree of reinforcement and ease of shifting to new responses in a Weigl-type card-sorting problem. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 38404-411.
Google Scholar
Happé, F. G. E. (1995). The role of age and verbal ability in the theory of mind task performance of subjects with autism. Child Development, 66843-855.
Google Scholar
Happé, F. G. E. (1996). Studying weak central coherence at low levels: Children with autism do not succumb to visual illusions. A research note. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 37873-877.
Google Scholar
Happé, F. G. E. (1997). Central coherence and theory of mind in autism: Reading homographs in context. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 151-12.
Google Scholar
Heaton, R. K., Chelune, G. J., Talley, J. L., Kay, G. G., & Curtiss, G. (1993). Wisconsin Card Sorting Test: Revised and expanded.Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.
Google Scholar
Howlin, P. (1997). Prognosis in autism: Do specialist treatments affect long-term outcome? European Child Psychiatry, 655-92.
Google Scholar
Hughes, C., Russell, J., & Robbins, T. W. (1994). Evidence for executive dysfunction in autism. Neuropsychologia, 32477-492.
Google Scholar
Jarrold, C., & Russell, J. (1997). Counting abilities in autism: Possible implications for central coherence theory. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 2725-37.
Google Scholar
Klin, A., Volkmar, F., & Sparrow, S. S. (1992). Autistic social dysfunction: Some limitations of the theory of mind hypothesis. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 33861-876.
Google Scholar
Lincoln, A. J., Courchesne, E., Kilman, B. A., Elmasian, R., & Allen, M. A. (1988). A study of intellectual abilities in high-functioning people with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 18505-524.
Google Scholar
McEvoy, R., Rogers, S., & Pennington, B. (1993). Executive function and social communication deficits in young autistic children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 34563-578.
Google Scholar
Muris, P., Steerneman, P., Meesters, C., Merckelbach, H., Horselenberg, R., van den Hogen, T., & van Dongen, L. (1999). The TOM test: A new instrument for assessing theory of mind in normal children and children with pervasive developmental disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 2967-80.
Google Scholar
Mulder, J. L., Dekker, R., & Dekker, P. H. (1996). Verbale leer en geheugen test.Lisse, The Netherlands; Swets & Zeitlinger.
Google Scholar
Navon, D. (1977). Forest before trees: The precedence of global features in visual perception. Cognitive Psychology, 9353-383.
Google Scholar
Nebelkopf, E. B., & Dreyer, A. S. (1970). Perceptual structuring: Cognitive domain differences in the perception of ambiguous stimuli. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 30635-639.
Google Scholar
Nordin, V., & Gillberg, C. (1998). The long-term course of autistic disorders: Update on follow-up studies. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 9799-108.
Google Scholar
Osterling, J., & Dawson, G. (1994). Early recognition of children with autism: A study of first birthday home videotapes. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 24247-258.
Google Scholar
Ozonoff, S. (1995). Reliability and validity of the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test in studies of autism. Neuropsychology, 9491-500.
Google Scholar
Ozonoff, S. (1997). Casual mechanisms of autism: Unifying perspectives from an information-processing framework. In D. J. Cohen, & F. R. Volkmar (Eds.), Handbook of autism and pervasive developmental disorders(pp. 868-879). New York: Wiley.
Google Scholar
Ozonoff, S., Pennington, B. F., & Rogers, S. J. (1991). Executive function deficits in high-functioning autistic individuals: Relationship to theory of mind. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 321081-1105.
Google Scholar
Ozonoff, S., & Strayer, D. L. (1997). Inhibitory function in nonretarded children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 2759-77.
Google Scholar
Ozonoff, S., Strayer, D. L., McHahon, W. M., & Filloux, F. (1994). Executive function abilities in autism and Tourette syndrome: an information processing approach. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 351015-1032.
Google Scholar
Ozonoff, S., & Miller, J. N. (1995). Teaching theory of mind: A new approach to social skills training for individuals with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 25415-433.
Google Scholar
Pascualvaca, D. M., Fantie, B. D., Papageorgiou, M., & Mirsky, A. F. (1998). Attentional capacities in children with autism: Is there a general deficit in shifting focus? Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 28467-478.
Google Scholar
Peterson, C., & Siegal, M. (1995). Deafness, conversation and theory of mind. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 36459-474.
Google Scholar
Pring, L., Hermelin, B., & Heavey, L. (1995). Savants, segments, art and autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 361065-1076.
Google Scholar
Prior, M., & Hoffman, W. (1990). Neuropsychological testing of autistic children through an exploration with frontal lobe tests: Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 20581-590.
Google Scholar
Shah, A., & Frith, U. (1983). An islet of ability in autistic children: a research note. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 24613-620.
Google Scholar
Shah, A., & Frith, U. (1993). Why do autistic individuals show superior performance on the block design task? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 341351-1364.
Google Scholar
Shulman, C., Yirmiya, N., & Greenbaum, C. W. (1995). From categorization to classification: a comparison among individuals with autism, mental retardation, and normal development.Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 104601-609.
Google Scholar
Sipps, G. J., Berry, G. W., & Lynch, E. M. (1987). WAIS-R and social intelligence: a test of established assumptions that uses the CPI. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 43499-504.
Google Scholar
Sparrevohn, R., & Howie, P. H. (1995). Theory of mind in children with autistic disorder: Evidence of developmental progression and the role of verbal ability. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 36249-263.
Google Scholar
Sparrow, S. S., Balla, D. A., & Cicchetti, D. V. (1984). Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales, Interview edition.Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.
Google Scholar
Tager-Flusberg, H. (1991). Semantic processing in the free recall of autistic children: Further evidence for a cognitive deficit. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 9417-430.
Google Scholar
Van Spaendonck, K. P. M., Berger, H. J. C., Horstink, M. W. I. M., Borm, G. F., & Cools, A. R. (1995). Card sorting performance in Parkinson's disease: a comparison between acquisition and shifting performance. Journal of Clinical Experimental Neuropsychology, 17918-925.
Google Scholar
Volkmar, F. R., Sparrow, S. S., Goudreau, E., Cicchetti, D. V., Paul, R., & Cohen, D. J. (1987). Social deficits in autism: An operational approach using the Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 26156-161.
Google Scholar
Warrington, E. K., & James, M. (1986). Visual object recognition in patients with right-hemisphere lesions: Axes or features? Perception, 15355-366.
Google Scholar
Warrington, E. K., & James, M. (1991). The Visual Object and Space Perception Battery.Bury St. Edmunds, UK: Thames Valley Test Co.
Google Scholar
Waterhouse, L., & Fein, D. (1984). Developmental trends in cognitive skills for children diagnosed as autistic and schizophrenic. Child Development, 55236-248.
Google Scholar
Wenar, C., & Ruttenberg, B. A. (1976). The use of BRIAC for evaluating therapeutic effectiveness. Journal of Autism and Childhood Schizophrenia, 6175-191.
Google Scholar
Witkin, H. A., Oltman, P. K., Raskin, E., & Karp, S. (1971). Manual for the Embedded Figures Test.Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.
Google Scholar