Advertisement

Political Behavior

, Volume 19, Issue 3, pp 177–196 | Cite as

Candidate Character vs. The Economy in the 1992 Election

  • Kathryn M. Doherty
  • James G. Gimpel
Article

Abstract

The outcome of the 1992 U.S. presidential election has been explained largely as a function of perceptions of George Bush's economic performance. The economy submerged questions about Bill Clinton's character, awarding the advantage to the Democrat. In this article, we evaluate the effect of economic evaluations along with character attacks on candidate support in the 1992 presidential contest. Claims that the economy submerged character have been somewhat exaggerated. But while character remains an important issue in presidential evaluation, its role in judging candidates cannot be taken at face value. We show that both economic evaluations and character judgments are highly politicized. The findings indicate that those protesting Clinton's character turned almost exclusively to Bush. Those protesting Bush's economic record turned to both Perot and Clinton. Still, the economy did not “trump” character. The troubles of both major party candidates fueled a strong protest vote that contributed to Perot's strong showing.

Keywords

Economic Evaluation Economic Performance Presidential Election Major Party Political Psychology 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

REFERENCES

  1. Alvarez, R. Michael, and Nagler, Jonathan (1995). Economics, issues and the Perot candidacy: Voter choice in the 1992 presidential election. American Journal of Political Science 39: 714-744.Google Scholar
  2. Axelrod, Robert (1973). Schema theory: An information processing model of perception and cognition. American Political Science Review 67: 1248-1266.Google Scholar
  3. Bartels, Larry M. (1988). Presidential Primaries and the Dynamics of Public Choice. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
  4. Bartels, Larry M. (1993). Messages received: The political impact of media exposure. American Political Science Review 87: 267-284.Google Scholar
  5. Beck, Nathaniel (1992). Forecasting the 1992 presidential election. Public Perspective 3 (September/October): 32-34.Google Scholar
  6. Berke, Richard L. (1992). Democrats charge “dirty tricks” over draft issue. New York Times, September 9, p. A10.Google Scholar
  7. Brody, Henry E., and Johnston, Richard (1987). What's the Primary Message: Horse Race or Issue Journalism? In Gary R. Orren and Nelson Polsby (eds.), Media and Momentum. New Jersey: Chatham House Press, pp. 127-187.Google Scholar
  8. Character issue hurts Clinton among affluent, suburban voters (1992). Gallup Poll Monthly (April), p. 15.Google Scholar
  9. Conover, Pamela Johnston (1988). Feminists and the gender gap. Journal of Politics 50: 985-1010.Google Scholar
  10. Conover, Pamela Johnston, and Feldman, Stanley (1989). Candidate perception in an ambiguous world: Campaigns, cues and inference processes. American Journal of Political Science 33: 912-940.Google Scholar
  11. Conover, Pamela Johnston, Feldman, Stanley, and Knight, Kathleen (1987). The personal and political underpinnings of economic forecasts. American Journal of Political Science 31: 559-583.Google Scholar
  12. Dowd, Maureen (1992). How a battered Clinton has stayed alive. New York Times, March 16, pp. A1, A15.Google Scholar
  13. Fiorina, Morris, and Shepsle, Kenneth A. (1989). Is negative voting an artifact? American Journal of Political Science 33: 423-439.Google Scholar
  14. Fiorina, Morris and Shepsle, Kenneth A. (1990). A positive theory of negative voting. In John Ferejohn and James Kuklinski (eds.), Information and Democratic Processes. Urbana: University of Illinois Press, pp. 219-239.Google Scholar
  15. Frankovic, Kathleen (1993). Public opinion in the 1992 campaign. In Gerald M. Pomper (ed.), The Election of 1992. Chatham, NJ: Chatham House, pp. 75-109.Google Scholar
  16. Gilens, Martin (1988). Gender and support for Reagan: A comprehensive model of presidential approval. American Journal of Political Science 32: 19-49.Google Scholar
  17. Glass, David P. (1985). Evaluating presidential candidates: Who focuses on their personal attributes? Public Opinion Quarterly 49: 517-534.Google Scholar
  18. Hamill, Ruth C., Lodge, Milton, and Blake, Frederick (1985). The breadth, depth and utility of class, partisan and ideological schemata. American Journal of Political Science 29: 850-870.Google Scholar
  19. Holm, John D., and Robinson, John P. (1978). Ideological identification and the American voter. Public Opinion Quarterly 42: 235-246.Google Scholar
  20. Hosmer, David W., and Lemeshow, Stanley (1989). Applied Logistic Regression. New York: John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
  21. Ifill, Gwen (1992a). Clinton defends his character to supporters. New York Times, April 20, p. A12.Google Scholar
  22. Ifill, Gwen (1992b). Clinton task: Making message heard. New York Times, February 21, p. A14.Google Scholar
  23. Jacoby, William G. (1988). The impact of party identification on issue attitudes. American Journal of Political Science 32: 643-661.Google Scholar
  24. Jacoby, William G. (1991). Ideological identification and issue attitudes. American Journal of Political Science 35: 178-205.Google Scholar
  25. Kelly, Michael (1992a). Bush subtly avoids direct attack against Clinton on the draft issue. New York Times, September 16, pp. A1, A19.Google Scholar
  26. Kelly, Michael (1992b). As race looks tighter, theme is truth and trust. New York Times, October 29, pp. A1, A23.Google Scholar
  27. Kinder, Donald R. (1985). Presidential character revisited. In Richard Lau and David O. Sears (eds.), Political Cognition. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
  28. Kinder, Donald R., Peters, Mark D., Abelson, Robert P., and Fiske, Susan T. (1980). Presidential prototypes. Political Behavior, 2: 315-337.Google Scholar
  29. Krosnick, Jon A. (1990). Americans' perceptions of presidential candidates: A test of the projection hypothesis. Journal of Social Issues 46: 159-182.Google Scholar
  30. Lau, Richard R. (1984). Two explanations for negativity effects in political behavior. American Journal of Political Science 29: 119-138.Google Scholar
  31. Lodge, Milton, and Hamill, Ruth (1986). A partisan schema for political information processing. American Political Science Review 80(2): 505-519.Google Scholar
  32. Lyons, William, and Scheb, John M. II (1992). Ideology and candidate evaluation in the 1984 and 1988 presidential elections. Journal of Politics 54: 573-584.Google Scholar
  33. MacKuen, Michael B., Erikson, Robert S., and Stimson, James A. (1992). Peasants or bankers? The American electorate in the U.S. economy. American Political Science Review 86: 597-611.Google Scholar
  34. Markus, Gregory B., and Converse, Philip (1979). A dynamic simultaneous equation model of electoral choice. American Political Science Review 73: 1055-70.Google Scholar
  35. McGrath, J., and McGrath, M. (1962). Effects of partisanship on perception of public figures. Public Opinion Quarterly 26: 236-248.Google Scholar
  36. Miller, Arthur H. (1986). Partisan cognitions in transition. In R. Lau and D. Sears (eds.), Political Cognition. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum and Assoc.Google Scholar
  37. Miller, Arthur H. (1993). Economic, character and social issues in the 1992 presidential election. American Behavioral Scientist 37: 315-327.Google Scholar
  38. Miller, Arthur H., and Wattenberg, Martin P. (1985). Throwing the rascals out: policy and performance evaluations of presidential candidates, 1952–1980. American Political Science Review 79: 359-372.Google Scholar
  39. Miller, Arthur H., Wattenberg, Martin P., and Malanchuk, Oksana (1986). Schematic assessments of presidential candidates. American Political Science Review 80: 521-540.Google Scholar
  40. Moon, David (1992). What you use still depends on what you have. American Politics Quarterly 20: 427-441.Google Scholar
  41. Moore, David W. (1987). Political campaigns and the knowledge-gap hypothesis. Public Opinion Quarterly 51: 186-200.Google Scholar
  42. Nelson, Michael (1993). The Elections of 1992. Washington: CQ Press.Google Scholar
  43. Page, Benjamin, and Jones, Calvin C. (1979). Reciprocal effects of policy preferences, party loyalties and the vote. American Political Science Review 73: 1071-1090.Google Scholar
  44. Perot benefits from Bush, Clinton weaknesses (1992). Gallup Poll Monthly, April, p. 18.Google Scholar
  45. Pomper, Gerald M. (1993). The Election of 1992. Chatham, NJ: Chatham House Publishers.Google Scholar
  46. Retherford, Robert D., and Choe, Minja Kim (1993). Statistical Models for Causal Analysis. New York: John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
  47. Riggle, Ellen (1993). Cognitive strategies and candidate evaluations. American Politics Quarterly 20: 227-246.Google Scholar
  48. Rosenthal, Andrew (1992). Bush steps up attacks on Democrat's character. New York Times, October 29, pp. A1, A24.Google Scholar
  49. Shapiro, Robert Y., and Mahajan, Harpreet (1986). Gender differences in policy preferences: A summary of trends from the 1960s to the 1980s. Public Opinion Quarterly 50: 42-61.Google Scholar
  50. Shepsle, Kenneth, and Fiorina, Morris (1990). In John Ferejohn and James H. Kuklinski (eds.), Information and Democratic Processes. Urbana: University of Illinois Press.Google Scholar
  51. Sigel, Roberta (1964). Effect of partisanship on the perception of political candidates. Public Opinion Quarterly 28: 483-496.Google Scholar
  52. Stoker, Laura (1993). Judging presidential character: The demise of Gary Hart. Political Behavior 15: 193-223.Google Scholar
  53. Stokes, Donald, and DiIulio, John J., Jr. (1993). The setting: Valence politics in modern elections. In Michael Nelson (ed.), The Elections of 1992. Washington: CQ Press, pp. 1-20.Google Scholar
  54. Toner, Robin (1992a). Clinton retains wide lead in latest survey. New York Times, September 16, pp. A1, A17.Google Scholar
  55. Toner, Robin (1992b). GOP looks at Clinton draft record and spies Willie Horton. New York Times, September 9, p. A1.Google Scholar
  56. Wattenberg, Martin P. (1991). The Rise of Candidate-Centered Politics. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  57. Weatherford, M. Stephen (1978). Economic conditions and electoral outcomes: Class differences in the political response to recession. American Journal of Political Science 22(4): 917-937.Google Scholar
  58. Wyer, Robert S., Jr., and Ottati, Victor C. (1993). Political information processing. In Shanto Iyengar and William McGuire (eds.), Explorations in Political Psychology. Durham, NC: Duke University Press.Google Scholar
  59. Wyer, Robert S., and Srull, T.K. (1989). Memory and Cognition in Its Social Context. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  60. Zaller, John (1991). Information, values and opinion. American Political Science Review 85: 1215-1223.Google Scholar
  61. Zaller, John (1992). The Nature and Origin of Mass Opinion. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Plenum Publishing Corporation 1997

Authors and Affiliations

  • Kathryn M. Doherty
  • James G. Gimpel
    • 1
  1. 1.Department of Government and PoliticsUniversity of MarylandCollege Park

Personalised recommendations