Journal of Science Education and Technology

, Volume 11, Issue 2, pp 135–144 | Cite as

How Interest in Science Negatively Influences Perceptions of Women

  • Sheila Brownlow
  • Tonya J. Smith
  • BeLinda R. Ellis
Article

Abstract

We examined whether women who pursue scientific training are viewed negatively by college students. Participants (N = 114 college students; 56 women, 58 men) saw a woman describe her major (either chemistry or the humanities) and express either strong commitment to her course of study or ambivalence about it. Participants then provided judgments about the speaker's traits (such as assertiveness and sociability), social behaviors (including how often she dates), and her future potential (including happiness and fulfillment with her career, as well as starting salary). Results indicated that both major and commitment level influenced perceptions of the speaker. Commitment to the major increased judgments of future career fulfillment, intellectual competence, school achievement, and assertiveness. Pursuit of chemistry, however, was perceived as negative, as people judged the woman as less sociable when she was committed to being a chemist, and women participants predicted she would have a less fulfilling career. Men participants said they would not want to date her. Although women may understand that they could make more money in the sciences, negative judgments about their social capabilities and future happiness may contribute to their propensity to avoid science.

female scientists sex-role stereotypes 

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

REFERENCES

  1. Acker, S., and Oatley, K. (1993). Gender issues in education for science and technology: Current situation and prospects for change. Canadian Journal of Education 18: 255-272.Google Scholar
  2. American Chemical Society (2001). Women Chemists 2000: Analysis of the American Chemical Society's Survey of the Salaries and Employment Status of its Domestic Members, American Chemical Society, Washington, DC.Google Scholar
  3. Archer, J. (1996). Sex differences in social behavior. American Psychologist 51: 909-917.Google Scholar
  4. Beller, M., and Gafni, N. (2000). Can item format (multiple choice vs. open-ended) account for gender differences in mathematics achievement? Sex Roles 42: 1-22.Google Scholar
  5. Beyer, S. (1995). Maternal employment and children's academic achievement: Parenting styles as mediating variables. Developmental Review 15: 212-253.Google Scholar
  6. Bodner, G. M., and Guay, R. B. (1997). The Purdue Visualization of Rotations Test. The Chemical Educator 2: 1-18.Google Scholar
  7. Brown, R. P., and Josephs, R.A. (1999). A burden of proof: Stereotype relevance and gender differences in math performance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 76: 246-257.Google Scholar
  8. Brownlow, S., and Durham, S. (1997). Sex differences in the use of science and technology in children's cartoons. Journal of Science Education and Technology 6: 103-110.Google Scholar
  9. Brownlow, S., Jacobi, T., and Rogers, M. I. (2000). Science anxiety as a function of gender and experience. Sex Roles 42: 119-131.Google Scholar
  10. Carter, C. S., LaRussa, M. A., and Bodner, G. M. (1987). A study of two measures of spatial ability as predictors of success in different levels of general chemistry. Journal of Research in Science Teaching 24: 645-657.Google Scholar
  11. Casey, M. B., Nuttall, R. L., and Pezaris, E. (1997). Mediators of gender differences in mathematics college entrance test scores: A comparison of spatial skills with internalized beliefs and anxieties. Developmental Psychology 33: 669-680.Google Scholar
  12. Coleman, S. L., and Gotch, A. J. (1998). Spatial perception skills of chemistry students. Journal of Chemical Education 75: 206-209.Google Scholar
  13. Colley, A., Comber,C., and Hargreaves, D. J. (1994). Gender effects in school subject preferences: A research note. Educational Studies 20: 13-18.Google Scholar
  14. Collis, B., and Ollila, L. (1990). The effect of computer use on grade 1 children's gender stereotypes about reading, writing, and computer use. Journal of Research and Development in Education 24: 2-8.Google Scholar
  15. Cooper, J., Hall, J., and Huff, C. (1990). Situational stress as a consequence of sex-typed software. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 16: 419-429.Google Scholar
  16. Dimitrov, D. M. (1999). Gender differences in science achievement: Differential effect of ability, response format, and strands of learning outcomes. School Science and Mathematics 99: 445-451.Google Scholar
  17. Fouad, N. A., and Smith, P. L. (1996). A test of a social cognitive model for middle school students: Math and Science. Journal of Counseling Psychology 43: 338-346.Google Scholar
  18. Furnham, A., and Rawles, R. (1995). Sex differences in the estimation of intelligence. Journal of Social Behavior and Personality 10: 741-748.Google Scholar
  19. Halpern, D. F. (1986). A different answer to the question, “do sex-related differences in spatial abilities exist?” American Psychologist 41: 1014-1015.Google Scholar
  20. Halpern, D. F. (1997). Sex differences in intelligence. American Psychologist 52: 1091-1102.Google Scholar
  21. Harris, A. M., and Carlton, S. T. (1993). Patterns of gender differences on mathematics items on the Scholastic Aptitude Test. Applied Measurement in Education 6: 137-151.Google Scholar
  22. Heatherington, L., Daubman, K. A., Bates, C., Ahn, A., Brown, H., and Preston,C. (1993). Two investigations of “female modesty” in achievement situations. Sex Roles 29: 739-754.Google Scholar
  23. Holden, C. (1987). Female math anxiety on the wane. Science 236: 660-661.Google Scholar
  24. Jacobs, J. E., and Eccles, J. S. (1992). The impact of mothers' genderrole stereotypic beliefs on mothers' and childrens' ability perceptions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 63: 932-944.Google Scholar
  25. Jussim, L., and Eccles, J. S. (1992). Teacher expectations. II: Construction and reflection of student achievement. Journal ofPersonality and Social Psychology 63: 947-961.Google Scholar
  26. Kahle, J. B., Parker, L. H., Rennie, L. J., and Riley, D. (1993). Gender differences in science education: Building a model. Educational Psychologist 28: 379-404.Google Scholar
  27. Kass, S. J., Ahlers, R. H., and Dugger, M. (1998). Eliminating gender differences through practice in an applied visual spatial task. Human Performance 11: 337-349.Google Scholar
  28. Kimball, M. M. (1989). A new perspective on women's math achievement. Psychological Bulletin 105: 198-214.Google Scholar
  29. Ledbetter, C. E. (1993). Qualitative comparison of students' constructions of science. Science Education 77: 611-624.Google Scholar
  30. Lent, R. W., Lopez, F. G., and Bieschke, K. J. (1991). Mathematics self-efficacy: Sources and relation to science-based career choices. Journal of Counseling Psychology 38: 424-430.Google Scholar
  31. Lips, H. M. (1992). Gender-and science-related attitudes as predictors of college students' academic choices. Journal of Vocational Behavior 40: 62-81.Google Scholar
  32. Lytton, H., and Romney, D. M. (1991). Parents' differential socialization of boys and girls: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin 109: 267-296.Google Scholar
  33. Mallow, J. V. (1994). Gender-related science anxiety: A first binational study. Journal of Science Education and Technology 3: 227-238.Google Scholar
  34. Paulsen, K., and Johnson, M. (1983). Sex role attitudes and mathematical ability in 4th-, 8th-, and 11th-grade students from a high socioeconomic area. Developmental Psychology 19: 210-214.Google Scholar
  35. Potts, R., and Martinez, I. (1994). Television viewing and children's beliefs about scientists. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology 15: 287-300.Google Scholar
  36. Pratto, F., Stallworth, L. M., Sidanius, J., and Siers, B. (1997). The gender gap in occupational role attainment: A social dominance approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 72: 37-53.Google Scholar
  37. Räsänen, L. (1991). Girls and the learning of physical concepts. The Finnish Journal of Education 22: 185-194.Google Scholar
  38. Rennie, L. J., and Dunne, M. (1994). Gender, ethnicity, and students' perceptions about science and science-related careers in Fuji. Science Education 78: 285-300.Google Scholar
  39. Seymour, E. (1995). The loss of women from science, mathematics, and engineering undergraduate majors: An explanatory account. Science Education 79: 437-473.Google Scholar
  40. Shepardson, D. P., and Pizzini, E. L. (1992). Gender bias in female elementary teachers' perceptions of the scientific ability of students. Science Education 76: 147-153.Google Scholar
  41. Signorella, M. L., and Jamison, W. (1986). Masculinity, femininity, androgyny, and cognitive performance: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin 100: 207-228.Google Scholar
  42. Smith, D. (2000). Women and minorities make gains in science and engineering education. Monitor on Psychology 31: 32.Google Scholar
  43. Smith, T. E. (1992). Gender differences in the scientific achievement of adolescents: Effects of age and parental separation. Social Forces 71: 469-483.Google Scholar
  44. Smith, A. E., Jussim, L., Eccles, J., VanNoy, M., Madon, S., and Palumbo, P. (1998). Self-fulfilling prophecies, perceptual biases, and accuracy at the individual and group levels. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 34: 530-561.Google Scholar
  45. Spencer, S. J., Steele, C. M., and Quinn, D. M. (1998). Stereotype threat and women's math performance. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 35: 4-28.Google Scholar
  46. Stricker, L. J., Rock, D. A., and Burton, N. W. (1993). Sex differences in predictions of college grades from Scholastic Aptitude Scores. Journal of Educational Psychology 85: 710-718.Google Scholar
  47. Terborg, J. R., and Ilgen, D. R. (1975). A theoretical approach to sex discrimination in traditionally masculine occupations. Organizational Behavior and Human Performance 13: 352-376.Google Scholar
  48. Trankina, M. L. (1993). Gender differences in attitudes toward science. Psychological Reports 73: 123-130.Google Scholar
  49. Voyer, D., Voyer, S., and Bryden, P. (1995). Magnitude of sex differences in spatial abilities:Ameta-analysis and consideration of critical variables. Psychological Bulletin 117: 250-270.Google Scholar
  50. Williams, J. E. (1994). Gender differences in high school students' efficacy expectation/performance discrepancies across four subject matter domains. Psychology in the Schools 31: 232-236.Google Scholar
  51. Williams, K. (2000/2001). Understanding, communication anxiety, and gender in physics. Journal of College Science Teaching 30: 232-237.Google Scholar
  52. Young, T. J. (1993). Women as comic book super heroes: The “weaker” sex in the Marvel universe. Psychology. A Journal of Human Behavior 30: 49-50.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Plenum Publishing Corporation 2002

Authors and Affiliations

  • Sheila Brownlow
    • 1
  • Tonya J. Smith
    • 1
  • BeLinda R. Ellis
    • 1
  1. 1.Department of PsychologyCatawba CollegeSalisbury

Personalised recommendations