Advertisement

Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders

, Volume 31, Issue 1, pp 29–36 | Cite as

Advanced Theory of Mind in High-Functioning Adults with Autism

  • Jamie Kleinman
  • Paul L. Marciano
  • Ruth L. Ault
Article

Abstract

Twenty-four high-functioning adults with autism (16 men) who passed a first-order theory-of-mind task and 24 nonautistic adults (10 men) attributed mental states to recordings of various verbal intonations and to photos of people's eyes to assess advanced theory of mind. Participants with autism performed significantly worse than nonautistic participants on both tasks. Thus, the previously described inattention to others' eyes exhibited by adults with autism is not solely responsible for their inability to attribute mental states from eyes, as they also did not correctly attribute mental states from voices. These findings support the view that a core deficit for people with autism lies in their theory of mind, that is, their inability to attribute mental states to others.

Autism theory of mind adults 

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

REFERENCES

  1. Bachorowski, J., & Owren, M. J. (1995). Vocal expressions of emotion: Acoustic properties of speech are associated with emotional intensity and content. Psychological Science, 6219-225.Google Scholar
  2. Banse, R., & Scherer, K. R. (1996). Acoustic profiles in vocal emotion expression. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70614-636.Google Scholar
  3. Baron-Cohen, S. (1989). The autistic child's theory of mind: A case of specific developmental delay. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 30285-297.Google Scholar
  4. Baron-Cohen, S. (1991). The development of a theory of mind in autism: Deviance and delay? Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 1433-51.Google Scholar
  5. Baron-Cohen, S., Baldwin, D. A., & Crowson, M. (1997). Do children with autism use the speaker's direction of gaze strategy to crack the code of language? Child Development, 6848-57.Google Scholar
  6. Baron-Cohen, S., Campbell, R., Karmiloff-Smith, A., Grant, J., & Walker, J. (1995). Are children with autism blind to the mentalistic significance of the eyes? British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 13379-398.Google Scholar
  7. Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A. M., & Frith, U. (1985). Does the autistic child have a “theory of mind”? Cognition, 2137-46.Google Scholar
  8. Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., & Jolliffe, T. (1997). Is there a “language of the eyes”? Evidence from normal adults and adults with autism or Asperger syndrome. Visual Cognition, 4311-331.Google Scholar
  9. Cosmides, L. (1983). Invariances in the acoustic expression of emotion during speech. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 9864-881.Google Scholar
  10. Dahlgren, S., & Trillingsgaard, A. (1996). Theory of mind in nonretarded children with autism and Asperger's syndrome: A research note. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 37759-763.Google Scholar
  11. Ekman, P., & Friesan, W. V. (1975). Unmasking the face: A guide to recognizing emotions from facial cues. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 931097-1108.Google Scholar
  12. Perner, J., Frith, U., Leslie, A. M., & Leekam, S. R. (1989). Exploration of the autistic child's theory of mind: Knowledge, belief, and communication. Child Development, 60689-700.Google Scholar
  13. Perner, J., Leekam, S. R., & Wimmer, H. (1987). Three-year-olds' difficulty with false belief: The case for a conceptual deficit. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 5125-137.Google Scholar
  14. Phillips, W., Baron-Cohen, S., & Rutter, M. (1992). The role of eye contact in goal detection: Evidence from normal infants and children with autism or mental handicap. Development and Psychopathology, 4375-383.Google Scholar
  15. Premack, D., & Woodruff, G. (1978). Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 1515-526.Google Scholar
  16. Prior, M., Dahlstrom, B., & Squires, T-L. (1990). Autistic children's knowledge of thinking and feeling states in other people. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 31587-601.Google Scholar
  17. Schopler, E., Reichler, R. J., & Renner, B. R. (1988). Childhood Autism Rating Scale.Chapel Hill, NC: Western Psychological Resources.Google Scholar
  18. Schultz, R. T., Gauthier, I., Klin, A., Fulbright, R., Anderson, A. W., Volkmar, F., Skudlarski, P., Lacadie, C., & Cohen, D. J. (1999). Abnormal ventral cortical activity among individuals with autism and Asperger Syndrome during face discrimination.Manuscript submitted for publication.Google Scholar
  19. Sparrevohn, R., & Howie, P. M. (1995). Theory of mind in children with autistic disorder: Evidence of developmental progression and the role of verbal ability. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 36249-263.Google Scholar
  20. Sparrow, S. S., Balla, D. A., & Cicchetti, D. V. (1984). Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scale.Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.Google Scholar
  21. Tager-Flusberg, H. (1992). Autistic children's talk about psychological states: Deficits in the early acquisition of a theory of mind. Child Development, 63161-172.Google Scholar
  22. Tonge, B. J., Dissanayake, C., & Brereton, A. V. (1994). Autism: Fifty years from Kanner. Journal of Paediatric Child Health, 30102-107.Google Scholar
  23. Volkmar, F. R., Szatmari, P., & Sparrow, S. S. (1993). Sex differences in pervasive developmental disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 23579-591.Google Scholar
  24. Wechsler, D. (1981). Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Revised.New York: Psychological Corp.Google Scholar
  25. Wimmer, H., & Perner, J. (1983). Beliefs about beliefs: Representation and constraining function of wrong beliefs in young children's understanding of deception. Cognition, 13103-128.Google Scholar
  26. Wiseman, R. (1995). The megalab truth test. Nature, 373391.Google Scholar
  27. Wolf-Schein, E. (1996). The autistic spectrum disorder: A current review. Developmental Disabilities Bulletin, 2433-55.Google Scholar
  28. Yirmiya, N., Erel, O., Shaked, M., & Solomonica-Levi, D. (1998). Meta-analyses comparing theory of mind abilities of individuals with autism, individuals with mental retardation, and normally developing individuals. Psychological Bulletin, 124283-307.Google Scholar
  29. Yirmiya, N., Solomonica-Levi, D., Shulman, C., & Pilowsky, T. (1996). Theory of mind abilities in individuals with autism, Down syndrome, and mental retardation of unknown etiology: The role of age and intelligence. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 371003-1014.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Plenum Publishing Corporation 2001

Authors and Affiliations

  • Jamie Kleinman
    • 1
  • Paul L. Marciano
    • 1
  • Ruth L. Ault
    • 1
  1. 1.Davidson CollegeDavidson

Personalised recommendations