Advertisement

Journal of Gambling Studies

, Volume 20, Issue 1, pp 1–21 | Cite as

The Stubborn Logic of Regular Gamblers: Obstacles and Dilemmas in Cognitive Gambling Research

  • Paul Delfabbro
Article

Abstract

Cognitive studies into gambling behaviour have shown that many gamblers misinterpret the odds of gambling activities, hold false beliefs about skill and luck, and over-estimate their subjective chances of success. Accordingly, providing accurate information about gambling in venues, or on slot-machines, would appear to be a sensible way to enhance the ‘safety’ of gambling products. Unfortunately, such ideas for educating gamblers may be difficult to implement in practice because of limitations and conceptual dilemmas in the cognitive literature. Irrational beliefs about gambling may be difficult to falsify, are often highly idiosyncratic and context-bound, and may stem more from the selective misuse of information than from a lack of knowledge about gambling activities. Drawing evidence from recent research into gambling-related cognitions, this paper examines the key elements of irrational-belief systems, and how this work might usefully inform the design of future consumer information initiatives.

cognitions problem gambling education 

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Anderson, G., & Brown, R.I.F. (1984). Real and laboratory gambling, sensation-seeking and arousal. British Journal of Psychology, 75, 405-410.Google Scholar
  2. Blaszczynski, A., Walker, M., Sagris, A., & Dickerson, M. (1997). Psychological aspects of gambling paper. Position paper prepared for the Directorate of Social Issues, Australian Psychological Society.Google Scholar
  3. Bouts, P. & van Avermaet, J. (1992). Drawing familiar or unfamiliar cards: stimulus familiarity, chance orientation, and the illusion of control. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 18, 331-335.Google Scholar
  4. Brown, R.I.F. (1986). Arousal and sensation-seeking components in the general explanation of gambling and gambling addictions. International Journal of the Addictions, 21, 1001-1016.Google Scholar
  5. Burger, J.M. (1991). The effects of desire for control in situations with chance determined outcomes: gambling behaviour in lotto and bingo players. Journal of Research in Personality, 25, 196-204.Google Scholar
  6. Cohen, J. (1972). Psychological probability or the art of doubt. London: George, Allen & Urwin.Google Scholar
  7. Comings, D. The molecular genetics of pathological gambling. CNS Spectrum, 3, 20-37.Google Scholar
  8. Corney, W.J., & Cummings, W.T. (1985). Gambling behavior and information processing biases. Journal of Gambling Behavior, 1, 111-118.Google Scholar
  9. Coventry, K., & Norman (1998). Arousal, erroneous verbalizations and the illusion of control during a computer-generated gambling task. British Journal of Psychology, 89, 629-645.Google Scholar
  10. Delfabbro, P.H. (1998). A psychological investigation of gambling in South Australia: with particular reference to the cognitive and behavioural determinants of regular poker/slot machine gambling. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, The University of Adelaide, South Australia.Google Scholar
  11. Delfabbro, P.H. (2000). Gender differences in Australian gambling: a critical summary of sociological and psychological research. Australian Journal of Social Issues, 35, 145-157.Google Scholar
  12. Delfabbro, P.H., & Winefield, A.H. (1999a). Poker machine gambling: an analysis of within session characteristics. British Journal of Psychology, 90, 425-439.Google Scholar
  13. Delfabbro, P.H., & Winefield, A.H. (1999b). The danger of over-explanation in psychological research. British Journal of Psychology, 90, 447-450.Google Scholar
  14. Delfabbro, P.H., & Winefield, A.H. (2000). Predictors of irrational thinking in slot-machine gambling. Journal of Psychology, 134, 17-28.Google Scholar
  15. Dickerson, M.G. (1984). Compulsive gamblers. London: Longman.Google Scholar
  16. Dixon, M.R., Hayes, L.J., & Ebbs, R.E. (1998). Engaging in “illusory control” during repeated risk-taking. Psychological Reports, 83, 959-962.Google Scholar
  17. Edwards, W. (1953). Probability preferences in gambling. American Journal of Psychology, 66, 349-364.Google Scholar
  18. Gaboury, A., & Ladouceur, R. (1988). Irrational thinking and gambling. In Eadington, W.R. (Ed.) Gambling research: proceedings of the seventh international conference on gambling and risk taking, Reno: University of Nevada.Google Scholar
  19. Gigerenzer, G. (1981). How to make cognitive illusions disappear: beyond heuristics and biases. European Review of Social Psychology, 2, 83-115.Google Scholar
  20. Gilovich, T. (1983). Biased evaluation and persistence in gambling. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 44, 1110-1126.Google Scholar
  21. Gilovich, T., & Douglas, C. (1986). Biased evaluations of randomly determined gambling outcomes. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 22, 228-241.Google Scholar
  22. Goldstein, L, & Carlton, P.L. (1988). Hemispheric EEG correlates of compulsive behavior: the case of pathological gamblers. Research Communications in Psychology, Psychiatry and Behavior, 13, 103-111.Google Scholar
  23. Griffiths, M.D. (1993). Fruit machine gambling: The importance of the structural characteristics. Journal of Gambling Studies, 9, 101-120.Google Scholar
  24. Griffiths, M.D. (1994). The role of cognitive bias and skill in fruit machine gambling. British Journal of Psychology, 85, 351-369.Google Scholar
  25. Griffiths, M.D. (1995). Adolescent gambling. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
  26. Griffiths, M.D., & Delfabbro, P.H. (2001). The biopsychosocial approach to the study of gambling. eGambling: The Electronic Journal of Gambling Issues (Feature article), 5, 1-33, www.camh.net/egambling.Google Scholar
  27. Gupta, R., & Derevensky, J. (1997). Familial and social influences on juvenile gambling behavior. Journal of Gambling Studies, 13, 179-192.Google Scholar
  28. Haroon, K., & Derevensky, J. (2001). Social influences involved in children's gambling behaviour. Journal of Gambling Studies, 17, 191-215.Google Scholar
  29. Henslin, J.M. (1967). Craps and magic. American Journal of Sociology, 73, 316-330.Google Scholar
  30. Hong, Y., & Chiu, C. (1988). Sex, locus of control, and illusion of control in Hong Kong as correlates of gambling involvement. Journal of Social Psychology, 128, 667-673.Google Scholar
  31. Keren, G., & Wagenaar, W.A. (1985). On the psychology of playing blackjack; normative and descriptive considerations with implications for decision theory. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 114, 133-158.Google Scholar
  32. Ladouceur, R., Mayrand, M., Dussault, R., Letarte, A., & Tremblay, J. (1984). Illusion of control: effects of participation and involvement, Journal of Psychology, 117, 47-52.Google Scholar
  33. Ladouceur, R., Tourigny, M., & Mayrand, M. (1986). Familiarity, group exposure and risk-taking in gambling. The Journal of Psychology, 120, 45-59.Google Scholar
  34. Ladouceur, R., Gaboury, A., Dumont, D, & Rochette, P. (1988). Gambling: relationship between the frequency of wins and irrational thinking. Journal of Psychology, 122, 409-414.Google Scholar
  35. Langer, E.J. (1975). The Illusion of Control. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 32, 311-328.Google Scholar
  36. Langer, E.J., & Roth, J. (1975). Heads you win, tails it's chance: the illusion of control as a function of the sequence of outcomes in a purely chance task. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 32, 951-955.Google Scholar
  37. Lo, R. (2000). Optimism and illusion of control in gambling. Journal of the National Association for Gambling Studies, 12, 18-23.Google Scholar
  38. Loba, P., Stewart, S.H., Klein, R.M., & Blackburn, J.R. (2002). Manipulations of the features of standard video lottery terminal (VLT) games: Effects in pathological and non-pathological gamblers, 297-320.Google Scholar
  39. Matute, H. (1995). Human reactions to uncontrollable outcomes: further evidence for superstitions rather than helplessness. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 48B, 142-157.Google Scholar
  40. Mok, W.P., & Hraba, J. (1991). Age and gambling behavior: a declining and shifting pattern of participation. pattern of participation. Journal of Gambling Studies, 7, 313-335.Google Scholar
  41. Productivity Commission (1999). Australia's gambling industries. Canberra: Federal Productivity Commission.Google Scholar
  42. Seligman, M.E.P., Abramson, L.Y., Semmel, A., & Von Baeyer, C. (1979). Depressive attributional style. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 88, 242-247.Google Scholar
  43. Skinner, B.F. (1948). “Superstition” in the pigeon. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 38, 168-172.Google Scholar
  44. Solonsch, M. (1991). The complete guide to Australian gambling. Wrightbooks Pty. Ltd., Victoria.Google Scholar
  45. Taylor, S.E., & Brown, J.D. (1988). Illusion and well-being: a social psychological perspective on mental health. Psychological Bulletin, 103, 193-210.Google Scholar
  46. Thompson, S.C., Armstrong, W., & Thomas, C. (1998). Illusions of control, underestimations, and accuracy: a control heuristic explanation. Psychological Bulletin, 123,143-161.Google Scholar
  47. Toneatto, T., Blitz-Miller, T, Calderwood, K., Dragonetti, R., & Tsanos, A. (1997). Cognitive distortions in heavy gambling. Journal of Gambling Studies, 13, 253-266.Google Scholar
  48. Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1973). Availability: a heuristic for judging frequency and probability. Cognitive Psychology, 5, 207-233.Google Scholar
  49. Tversky, A., Kahneman, D., & Slovic, P. (1982). Judgement under uncertainty: heuristics and biases. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  50. Wagenaar, W.A. (1988). Paradoxes of gambling behaviour. England: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  51. Walker, M.B. (1992a). The Psychology of Gambling, Pergamon Press, Sydney.Google Scholar
  52. Walker, M.B. (1992b). Irrational thinking among slot machine players. Journal of Gambling Studies, 8, 245-288.Google Scholar
  53. Walker, M.B., Sturevska, S., & Turpie, D. (1995). The quality of play in Australian casinos. Paper presented at the seventh conference of the National Association for Gambling Studies, Fremantle, Western Australia.Google Scholar
  54. Weinstein, N.D. (1980). Unrealistic optimism about future life events. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39, 806-820.Google Scholar
  55. Weinstein, N.D., & Klein, W.M. (1996). Unrealistic optimism: present and future. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 15, 1-8.Google Scholar
  56. Winefield, A.H. (1966). Negative recency and event dependence. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 18, 47-54.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Human Sciences Press, Inc. 2004

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of PsychologyUniversity of AdelaideNth. Tce.

Personalised recommendations