A Systematic Review of Action Imitation in Autistic Spectrum Disorder

  • Justin H. G. Williams
  • Andrew Whiten
  • Tulika Singh
Article

Abstract

Imitative deficits have been associated with autistic spectrum disorder (ASD) for many years, most recently through more robust methodologies. A fresh, systematic review of the significance, characteristics, and underlying mechanism of the association is therefore warranted. From 121 candidates, we focused on 21 well-controlled studies involving 281 cases of ASD. Overall, children with ASD performed worse on imitative tasks (Combined Logit p value < .00005). The emerging picture is of delayed development in imitation, implicating a deficit in mapping neural codings for actions between sensory and motor modalities, rather than in motivation or executive function. We hypothesise that ASD is characterised by abnormal development of these mappings, such that they are biased towards object-oriented tasks at the expense of those required for action imitation per se.

Imitation gesture action autism emulation ‘mirror neurons’ 

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Aldridge, M. A., Stone, K. R., Sweeney, M. H., & Bower, T.G.R. (2000). Preverbal children with autism understand the intentions of others. Developmental Science, 3, 294-301.Google Scholar
  2. Bailey, A., Luthert, P., Dean, A., Harding, B., Janota, I., Montgomery, M., Rutter, M., & Lantos, P. (1998). A clinicopathological study of autism. Brain, 121(Pt 5), 889-905.Google Scholar
  3. Baron-Cohen, S. (1989). The autistic child's theory of mind: A case of specific developmental delay. Journal of Child Psychology & Psychiatry, 30, 285-297.Google Scholar
  4. Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A. M., & Frith, U. (1985). Does the autistic child have a “theory of mind”? Cognition, 21, 37-46.Google Scholar
  5. Bartak, L., Rutter, M., & Cox, A. (1975). A comparative study of infantile autism and specific development receptive language disorder. I. The children. British Journal of Psychiatry, 126, 127-145.Google Scholar
  6. Britton, A., McKee, M., Black, N., McPherson, K., Sanderson, C., & Bain, C. (1998). Simple methods for combining studies. Choosing between randomised and non-randomised studies: A systematic review. Health Technol Assessment, 2(13), 33-38.Google Scholar
  7. Brown, J. 1996. Imitation, play and theory of mind in autism: An observational and experimental study. Unpublished PhD thesis. University of St. Andrews.Google Scholar
  8. Byrne, R. W., & Russon, A. E. (1998). Learning by imitation: A hierarchical approach. Behavioural Brain Science, 21, 667-684.Google Scholar
  9. Carruthers, P., & Smith, P. K. (1996). Introduction. Theories of Mind pp. 1-8.Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  10. Cermak, S. A., Coster, W., & Drake, C. (1980). Representational and nonrepresentational gestures in boys with learning disabilities. American Journal of Occupational Therapy, 34, 19-26.Google Scholar
  11. Charman, T., & Baron-Cohen S. (1994)Another look at imitation in autism. Developmental Psychopathology, 6, 403-413.Google Scholar
  12. Charman, T., Swettenham, J., Baron-Cohen, S., Cox, A., Baird, G., & Drew, A. (1997). Infants with autism: An investigation of empathy, pretend play, joint attention, and imitation. Developmental Psychology, 33, 781-789.Google Scholar
  13. Charman, T., Swettenham, J., Baron-Cohen, S., Cox, A., Baird, G., & Drew, A. (1998). An experimental investigation of social-cognitive abilities in infants with autism: Clinical implications. Infant Mental Health Journal, 19, 260-275.Google Scholar
  14. Curcio, F. (1978). Sensorimotor functioning and communication in mute autistic children. Journal of Autism and Child Schizophrenia, 8, 281-292.Google Scholar
  15. Dautenhahn, K., & Nehaniv, C. L. (2002). Imitation in animals and artifacts Cambridge, MA USA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  16. Dawson, B. V., & Foss, B. M. (1965). Observational learning in budgerigars. Animal Behaviour, 13, 470-474.Google Scholar
  17. DeMeyer, M. K., Alpern, G. D., Barton, S., DeMyer, W. E., Churchill, D. W., Hingtgen, J. N., Bryson, C. Q., Pontius, W., & Kimberlin, C. (1972). Imitation in autistic, early schizophrenic, and non-psychotic subnormal children. Journal of Autism and Child Schizophrenia, 2, 264-287.Google Scholar
  18. Fatemi, S. H., & Halt, A. R. (2001). Altered levels of Bcl2 and p53 proteins in parietal cortex reflect deranged apoptotic regulation in autism. Synapse, 42, 281-284.Google Scholar
  19. Fatemi, S. H., Halt, A. R., Stary, J. M., Realmuto, G. M., & Jalali-Mousavi, M. (2001). Reduction in anti-apoptotic protein Bcl-2 in autistic cerebellum. Neuroreport, 12, 929-933.Google Scholar
  20. Gallese, V., Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L., & Rizzolatti, G. (1996). Action recognition in the premotor cortex. Brain, 119, 593-609.Google Scholar
  21. Gallese, V., & Goldman, A. (1998). Mirror neurons and thesimulation theory of mind-reading. Trends in Cognitive Science, 2, 493-501.Google Scholar
  22. Green, D., Baird, G., Barnett, A. L., Huber, J., Henderson, S. E., (in press) The severity and nature of motor impairment in Asperger's Syndrome: A comparison with Specific Developmental Disorder of Motor Function.Developmental Disorder of Motor Function.Journal of Child, 43, 655-668.Google Scholar
  23. Griffith, E. M., Pennington, B. F., Wehner, E. A., & Rogers, S. J. (1999).Executive functions in young children with autism. Child Development, 70, 817-832.Google Scholar
  24. Hammes, J. G., & Langdell, T. (1981). Precursors of symbol formation and childhood autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorder, 11, 331-346.Google Scholar
  25. Hayes, K. J., & Hayes, C. (1952). Imitation in a home-reared chimpanzee. Journal of Comparitive Psychology, 45, 450-459.Google Scholar
  26. Hobson, R. P., & Lee, A. (1999). Imitation and identification in autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 40, 649-659.Google Scholar
  27. Hughes, C. (1996). Brief report: Planning problems in autism at the level of motor control. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorder, 26, 99-107.Google Scholar
  28. Jones, V., & Prior, M. (1985)Motor imitation abilities and neurological signs in autistic children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorder, 15, 37-46.Google Scholar
  29. Kanner, L. (1943)Autistic disturbances of affective content. Nervous Child, 2, 217-250.Google Scholar
  30. Libby, S., Powell, S., Messer, D., & Jordan, R. (1997). Imitation of pretend play acts by children with autism and Down syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorder, 27, 365-383.Google Scholar
  31. Lord, C., Risi, S., Lambrecht, L., Cook, E. H., Jr., Leventhal, B. L., DiLavore, P. C., Pickles, A., & Rutter, M. (2000). The autism diagnostic observation schedule-generic: A standard measure of social and communication deficits associated with the spectrum of autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorder, 30, 205-223.Google Scholar
  32. Lord, C., Rutter, M., & Le Couteur, A. (1994). Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised: A revised version of a diagnostic interview for caregivers of individuals with possible pervasive developmental disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorder, 24, 659-685.Google Scholar
  33. McDonough, L., Stahmer, A., Schreibman, L., & Thompson, S. J. (1997). Deficits, delays, and distractions: An evaluation of symbolic play and memory in children with autism. Developmental Psychopathology, 9, 17-41.Google Scholar
  34. Meltzoff, A. N. (1988a). Infant imitation after a 1-week delay: long term memory for novel acts and multiple stimuli. Developmental Psychology, 24, 470-476.Google Scholar
  35. Meltzoff, A. N. (1988b). Infant imitation and memory: Nine-month-olds in immediate and deferred tests. Child Development, 59, 217-225.Google Scholar
  36. Meltzoff, A. N. (1995). Understanding of the intentions of others: Re-enactment of intended acts by 18 month-old children. Developmental Psychology, 31, 838-850.Google Scholar
  37. Meltzoff, A. N., & Prinz, W. (Eds.) (2002). The imitative mind: Development, evolution and brain bases Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  38. Morgan, S. B., Cutrer, P. S., Coplin, J. W., & Rodrigue, J. R. (1989). Do autistic children differ from retarded and normal children in Piagetian sensorimotor functioning? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 30, 857-864.Google Scholar
  39. Ohta, M. (1987). Cognitive disorders of infantile autism: A study employing the WISC, spatial relationship conceptualization, and gesture imitations. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorder, 17, 45-62.Google Scholar
  40. Ritvo, S., & Provence, S. (1953). Form perception and imitation in some autistic children: Diagnostic findings and their contextual interpretation. The Psychoanalytical Study of the Child, 8, 155-161.Google Scholar
  41. Rizzolatti, G., Fogassi, L., & Gallese, V. (2001). Neurophysiological mechanisms underlying the understanding and imitation of action. Nature Reviews in Neuroscience, 2, 661-670.Google Scholar
  42. Rizzolatti, G., Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L., & Gallese, V. (2002). From mirror neurons to imitation: Facts and speculations. In A. Meltzoff & W. Prinz (Eds.), The imitative mind: Development, evolution and brain bases. (pp. 247-290). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  43. Royeurs, H., Van Oost, P., & Bothuyne, S. (1998). Immediate imitation and joint attention in young children with autism. Developmental Psychopathology, 10, 441-450.Google Scholar
  44. Rogers, S. J. (1999). An examination of the imitation deficit in autism. In J. Nadel, & G. Butterworth (Eds.), Imitation in infancy. (pp. 254-283). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  45. Rogers, S. J., Bennetto, L., McEvoy, R., & Pennington, B. F. (1996). Imitation and pantomime in high-functioning adolescents with autism spectrum disorders. Child Development, 67, 2060-2073.Google Scholar
  46. Rogers, S. J., & Pennington, B. F. (1991). A theoretical approach to the deficits in infantile autism. Developmental Psychopathology, 3, 137-162.Google Scholar
  47. Sigman, M., & Ungerer, J. A. (1984). Cognitive and language skills in autistic, mentally retarded and normal children. Developmental Psychology, 20, 293-302.Google Scholar
  48. Smith, G. D., & Egger, M. (1999). Meta-analyses of observational data should be done with due care. British Medical Journal, 318, 56.Google Scholar
  49. Smith, I. M., & Bryson, S. E. (1994). Imitation and action in autism: A critical review. Psychological Bulletin, 116, 259-273.Google Scholar
  50. Smith, I. M., & Bryson, S. E. (1998). Gesture Imitation in autism I: Nonsymbolic postures and sequences. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 15, 747-770.Google Scholar
  51. Stern, D. N. (1985). The interpersonal world of the human infant. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
  52. Stone, W. L., Lemanek, K. L., Fishel, P. T., Fernandez, M. C., & Altemeier, W. A. (1990). Play and imitation skills in the diagnosis of autism in young children. Pediatrics, 86, 267-272.Google Scholar
  53. Stone, W. L., Ousley, O. Y., & Littleford, C. D. (1997). Motor imitation in young children with autism: What's the object? Journal of Abnormalities in Child Psychology, 25, 475-485.Google Scholar
  54. Tantam, D. (1991). Asperger Syndrome in adulthood. In U. Frith (Eds.), Autism and asperger syndrome. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  55. Trevarthen, C., & Aitken K. J. (2001). Infant intersubjectivity: Research, theory, and clinical applications. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 42, 3-48.Google Scholar
  56. Uzgiris, I. C., & Hunt, J. M. (1975). Assessment in infancy. Urbana: University of Illinois Press.Google Scholar
  57. Vogt, S. (2002). Visuomotor couplings in object-oriented and imitative actions. In A. Meltzoff & W. Prinz, (Eds.), The imitative mind: development, evolution and brain bases. (pp. 206-220). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  58. Want, S. C., & Harris, P. L. (2002). How do children ape? Applying concepts from the study of non-human primates to the developmental study of ‘imitation’ in children. Developmental Science, 5(1), 1-41.Google Scholar
  59. Wetherby, A. M., & Prutting, C. A. (1984). Profiles of communicative and cognitive-social abilities in autistic children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 27, 364-377.Google Scholar
  60. Whiten, A., & Brown, J. (1999). Imitation and the reading of other minds: Perspectives from the study of autism, normal children and non-human primates. In S. Braten (Ed.), Intersubjective communication and emotion in early ontogenya. (pp. 260-280). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  61. Whiten, A., Custance, D. M., Gomez, J. C., Teixidor, P., & Bard, K. A. (1996). Imitative learning of artificial fruit processing in children (Homo sapiens) and chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Journal of Comparitive Psychology, 110, 3-14.Google Scholar
  62. Whiten, A., & Ham, R. (1992). On the nature and evolution of imitation in the animal kingdom: Reappraisal of a century of research. In P. J. B. Slater, C. Rosenblatt & M. Milinski (Eds.), Advances in the study of behaviour (Vol. 21 pp. 239-283).Google Scholar
  63. Williams, J. H., Whiten, A., Suddendorf, T., & Perrett, D. I. (2001). Imitation, mirror neurons and autism. Neuroscience and Biobehaviour Review, 25, 287-295.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Plenum Publishing Corporation 2004

Authors and Affiliations

  • Justin H. G. Williams
    • 1
  • Andrew Whiten
    • 2
  • Tulika Singh
    • 3
  1. 1.Department of Child HealthUniversity of Aberdeen, Medical School, ForesterhillAberdeenUK
  2. 2.School of Psychology, University of St.AndrewsSt.AndrewsUK
  3. 3.Torbay HospitalTorbay

Personalised recommendations