Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology

, Volume 32, Issue 3, pp 305–320 | Cite as

Is There a Dark Side of Positive Illusions? Overestimation of Social Competence and Subsequent Adjustment in Aggressive and Nonaggressive Children

  • Mara Brendgen
  • Frank Vitaro
  • Lyse Turgeon
  • François Poulin
  • Brigitte Wanner
Article

Abstract

This study examined (a) the links between children's overly positive perceptions about the relations with the peer group and with their best friend to subsequent behavioral, emotional, and social adjustment, and (b) whether these links are moderated by children's aggression. Using a short-term longitudinal design, the study was based on a sample of 819 4th- to 6th-graders (427 girls) from low to average SES families. Results showed that positive illusions about their social relations with classmates and with friends were related to an increase in children's peer-rated social preference and fostered the stability of children's dyadic friendships, regardless of children's level of aggression. In addition, overestimation of social competence with the peer group and overestimation of friendship quality were both related to a decrease in children's depressive feelings. Extreme overestimation as well as extreme underestimation of social competence with the peer group was also related to an increase in children's aggression. This latter result, however, was only true for children who were highly aggressive to begin with. The theoretical and practical implications of these results are discussed.

positive illusions social acceptance friendship quality aggression depression 

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

REFERENCES

  1. Abramson, L. Y., Seligman, M. E. P., & Teasdale, J. D. (1978). Learned helplessness in humans: Critique and reformulation. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 87, 49-74.Google Scholar
  2. Bandura, A. (1997). Self-efficacy: The exercise of control. New York: W. H. Freeman.Google Scholar
  3. Baumeister, R. F., Bushman, B. J., & Campbell, W. K. (2000). Self-esteem, narcissism, and aggression: Does violence result from low self-esteem or from threatened egotism? Current Directions in Psychological Science, 9, 26-29.Google Scholar
  4. Baumeister, R. F., Smart, L., & Boden, J. M. (1996). Relation of threatened egotism to violence and aggression: The dark side of high self esteem. Psychological Review, 103, 5-33.Google Scholar
  5. Beck, A. T. (1963). Thinking and depression: Idiosyncratic content and cognitive distortions. Archives of General Psychiatry, 9, 324-333.Google Scholar
  6. Berkowitz, L. (1978). Whatever happened to the frustration-aggression hypothesis? American Behavioral Scientist, 21, 691-707.Google Scholar
  7. Berkowitz, L. (1989). Frustration-aggression hypothesis: Examination and reformulation. Psychological Bulletin, 106, 59-73.Google Scholar
  8. Bjoerkqvist, K., Oesterman, K., & Kaukiainen, A. (1992). The development of direct and indirect aggressive strategies in males and females. In K. Bjoerkqvist & P. Niemelae (Eds.), Of mice and women: Aspects of female aggression (pp. 51-64). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  9. Boivin, M., & Begin, G. (1989). Peer status and self-perception among early elementary school children: The case of the rejected children. Child Development, 60, 591-596.Google Scholar
  10. Boivin, M., & Poulin, F. (1993). Les camarades de jeu des garcons agressifs. Les choix de camarades de jeu et la qualite de l'insertion sociale des garcons agressifs. Enfance, 47, 261-278.Google Scholar
  11. Boivin, M., Vitaro, F., & Gagnon, C. (1992). A reassessment of the self-perception profile for children: Factor structure, reliability, and convergent validity of a French version among second through sixth grade children. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 15, 275-290.Google Scholar
  12. Brendgen, M., Little, T. D., & Krappmann, L. (2000). Rejected children and their friends: A shared evaluation of friendship quality? Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 46, 45-70.Google Scholar
  13. Brendgen, M., Markiewicz, D., Doyle, A. B., & Bukowski, W. M. (2001). The relations between friendship quality, ranked friendship preference, and adolescents' behavior with their friends. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 47, 395-415.Google Scholar
  14. Brendgen, M., Vitaro, F., Turgeon, L., & Poulin, F. (2002) Assessing aggressive and depressed children's social relations with classmates and friends: A matter of perspective. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 30, 609-624.Google Scholar
  15. Cillessen, A. H. N., & Bellmore, A. D. (1999). Accuracy of social self-perceptions and peer competence in middle childhood. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 45, 650-676.Google Scholar
  16. Coie, J. D., Dodge, K. A., & Coppotelli, H. (1982). Dimensions and types of social status: A cross-age perspective. Developmental Psychology, 18, 557-570.Google Scholar
  17. Cole, D. A., Martin, J. M., Peeke, L. A., Seroczynski, A. D., & Fier, J. (1999). Children's over-and underestimation of academic competence: A longitudinal study of gender differences, depression, and anxiety. Child Development, 70, 459-473.Google Scholar
  18. Cole, D. A., Martin, J. M., Peeke, L. G., Seroczynski, A. D., & Hoffman, K. (1998). Are cognitive errors of underestimation predictive or reflective of depressive symptoms in children: A longitudinal study. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 107, 481-496.Google Scholar
  19. Colvin, C. R., Block, J., & Funder, D. C. (1995). Overly positive self-evaluations and personality: Negative implications for mental health. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68, 1152-1162.Google Scholar
  20. Crick, N. R., Werner, N. E., Casas, J. F., O'Brien, K. M., Nelson, D. A., Grotpeter, J. K., et al. (1999). Childhood aggression and gender: A new look at an old problem. In D. Bernstein (Ed.), Gender and motivation. Nebraska symposium on motivation (Vol. 45, pp. 75-141). Lincoln, NE: University of Nebraska Press.Google Scholar
  21. Cronbach, L. J., & Furby, L. (1970). How we should measure "change"—or should we? Psychological Bulletin, 74, 68-80.Google Scholar
  22. David, D. F., & Kistner, J. A. (2000). Do positive self-perceptions have a "dark side"? Examination of the link between perceptual bias and aggression. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 28, 327-337.Google Scholar
  23. Dodge, K. A., & Coie, J. D. (1987). Social-information processing factors in reactive and proactive aggression in children's peer groups. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53, 1146-1158.Google Scholar
  24. Finch, A. J., Saylor, C. F., & Edwards, G. L. (1985). Children's Depression Inventory: Gender and grade norms for normal children. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 53, 424-425.Google Scholar
  25. Fundulis, T., Berney, T. P., Kolvin, O., Famuyiva, O. O., Barrett, T., Bhate, S., et al. (1991). Reliability and validity of two rating scales in the assessment of childhood depression. British Journal of Psychiatry, 159, 36-40.Google Scholar
  26. Furman, W. (1996). The measurement of friendship perceptions: Conceptual and methodological issues. In W. M. Bukowski, A. F. Newcomb, & W. W. Hartup (Eds.), The company they keep: Friendship in childhood and adolescence (pp. 41-65). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  27. Harter, S. (1985). The self-perception profile for children: Revision of the perceived competence scale for children. Manual, University of Denver, Denver, CO.Google Scholar
  28. Harter, S. (1990). Developmental differences in the nature of self-representations: Implications for the understanding, assessment, and treatment of maladaptive behavior. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 14, 113-142.Google Scholar
  29. Hartup, W. W. (1992). Friendships and their developmental significance. In H. McGurk (Ed.), Childhood social development: Contemporary perspectives (pp. 175-205). Hillsdale, NJ, Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  30. Hoffman, K. B., Cole, D. A., Martin, J. M., Tram, J., & Seroczynski, A. D. (2000). Are the discrepancies between self-and others' appraisals of competence predictive or reflective of depressive symptoms in children and adolescents: A longitudinal study, part II. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 109, 651-662.Google Scholar
  31. Hoza, B., Pelham, W. E., Dobbs, J., Owens, J. S., & Pillow, D. R. (2002). Do boys with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder have positive illusory self-concepts? Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 111, 268-278.Google Scholar
  32. Hughes, J. N., Cavell, T. A., & Prasad-Gaur, A. (2001). A positive view of social preference in aggressive youth risk for future social preference. Journal of School Psychology, 39, 239-252.Google Scholar
  33. Hymel, S., Bowker, A., & Woody, E. (1993). Aggressive versus withdrawn unpopular children: Variations in peer and self-perceptions in multiple domains. Child Development, 64, 879-896.Google Scholar
  34. Hymel, S., Franke, S., & Freigang, R. (1985). Peer relationships and their dysfunction: Considering the child's perspective. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 3, 405-415.Google Scholar
  35. Jaccard, J., Turrisi, R., & Wan, C. K. (1990). Interaction effects in multiple regression (Sage series: Quantitative applications in the social sciences). London: Sage.Google Scholar
  36. Kovacs, M. (1985). The Children's Depression Inventory. Psychopharmacology Bulletin, 21, 995-998.Google Scholar
  37. Krantz, M., & Burton, C. (1986). The development of the social cognition of social status. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 147, 89-95.Google Scholar
  38. Kruglanski, A. W. (1989). The psychology of being "right": The problem of accuracy in social perception and cognition. Psychological Bulletin, 106, 395-409.Google Scholar
  39. McGrath, E. P., & Repetti, R. L. (2002). A longitudinal study of children's depressive symptoms, self-perceptions, and cognitive distortions about the self. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 111, 77-87.Google Scholar
  40. Menard, S. (2001). Applied logistic regression analysis (Sage University paper series on quantitative applications in the social sciences, 07–106). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
  41. O'Connell, P., Pepler, D., & Craig, W. (1999). Peer involvement in bullying: Insights and challenges for intervention. Journal of Adolescence, 22, 437-452.Google Scholar
  42. Panak, W. F., & Garber, J. (1992). Role of aggression, rejection, and attributions in the prediction of depression in children. Development and Psychopathology, 4, 145-165.Google Scholar
  43. Parker, J. G., & Asher, S. R. (1993). Friendship and friendship quality in middle childhood: Links with peer group acceptance and feelings of loneliness and social dissatisfaction. Developmental Psychology, 29, 611-621.Google Scholar
  44. Parkhurst, J. T., & Asher, S. R. (1992). Peer rejection in middle school: Subgroup differences in behavior, loneliness, and interpersonal concerns. Developmental Psychology, 28, 231-241.Google Scholar
  45. Patterson, C. J., Kupersmidt, J. B., & Griesler, P. C. (1990). Children's perceptions of self and of relationships with others as a function of sociometric status. Child Development, 61, 1335-1349.Google Scholar
  46. Pekarik, E. G., Prinz, R. J., Liebert, D. E., Weintraub, S., & Neale, J. M. (1976). The Pupil Evaluation Inventory: A sociometric technique for assessing children's social behavior. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 4, 83-97.Google Scholar
  47. Rabiner, D. & Coie, J. (1989). Effect of expectancy inductions on rejected children's acceptance by unfamiliar peers. Developmental Psychology, 25, 450-457.Google Scholar
  48. Rehm, L. P. (1977). A self-control model of depression. Behavior Therapy, 8, 787-804.Google Scholar
  49. Rubin, K. H., Hymel, S., Lemare, L., & Rowden L. (1989). Children experiencing social difficulties: Sociometric neglect reconsidered. Canadian Journal of Behavioural Science, 21, 94-111.Google Scholar
  50. Sandstrom, M. J. & Coie, J. D. (1999). A developmental perspective on peer rejection: Mechanisms of stability and change. Child Development, 70, 955-966.Google Scholar
  51. Taylor, S. E., & Brown, J. D. (1988). Illusions and well-being: A social psychological perspective on mental health. Psychological Bulletin, 103, 193-210.Google Scholar
  52. Taylor, S. E., & Brown, J. D. (1994). Positive illusions and well-being revisited: Separating fact from fiction. Psychological Bulletin, 116, 21-27.Google Scholar
  53. Vallerand, R. J. (1989). Vers une methodologie de validation trans-culturelle de questionnaires psychologiques: Implications pour la recherche en langue francaise [Toward a methodology for the transcultural validation of psychological questionnaires: Implications for research in the French language]. Canadian Psychology, 30, 662-680.Google Scholar
  54. Vernberg, E. M. (1990). Psychological adjustment and experiences with peers during early adolescence: Reciprocal, incidental, or unidirectional relationships? Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 18, 187-198.Google Scholar
  55. Zakriski, A., & Coie, J. D. (1996). A comparison of aggressive-rejected and nonaggressive-rejected children's interpretations of self-directed and other-directed rejection. Child Development, 7, 1048-1070.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Plenum Publishing Corporation 2004

Authors and Affiliations

  • Mara Brendgen
    • 1
  • Frank Vitaro
    • 2
  • Lyse Turgeon
    • 3
  • François Poulin
    • 1
  • Brigitte Wanner
    • 2
  1. 1.Department of PsychologyUniversité du Québec à MontréalMontrealCanada
  2. 2.Research Unit on Children's Psychosocial MaladjustmentUniversity of MontrealMontrealCanada
  3. 3.School of PsychoeducationUniversity of MontrealMontrealCanada

Personalised recommendations