Advertisement

International Journal of Primatology

, Volume 25, Issue 1, pp 41–54 | Cite as

Habitat Quality and Activity Budgets of White-Headed Langurs in Fusui, China

  • Zhaoyuan Li
  • Elizabeth Rogers
Article

Abstract

Within a species, habitat quality may be a factor causing different activity budgets between populations. The habitat of white-headed langurs (Trachypithecus leucocephalus) has been seriously disturbed in Fusui Rare and Precious Animal Nature Reserve, China, where we carried out a study of their socioecology from September 1997 to September 1998. We collected data on langur activity budgets from the main population located in the central part of a group of limestone hills. We classified habitat quality into 4 grades according to the extent of human disturbance. We showed that the two main study groups of white-headed langurs spent on average 50% of time resting, 13% feeding, 18% moving (including foraging), 11% grooming, and 7% playing. Langur time budgets showed no significant seasonal change, but they differed among different sex-age classes. Infants and juveniles spent about 20.3% of time playing, whereas adults spent only 0.2% playing. The group in high quality habitat engaged less in feeding and more in playing than the group in low quality habitat did. Habitat quality influenced the playing time of young white-headed langurs and may be vital to their successful maturation.

time budget habitat quality Trachypithecus leucocephalus conservation China 

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Reference

  1. Altmann, J. (1974). Observational study of behavior: Sampling methods. Behaviour 49: 227-267.Google Scholar
  2. Bercovitch, F. B. (1983). Time budgets and consortships in olive baboons (Papio anubis). Folia Primatol 41: 180-190.Google Scholar
  3. Burghardt, G. M. (1984). On the origins of play. In Smith, P. K. (ed.), Play in Animals and Humans, Basil Blackwell, Oxford, pp. 5-42.Google Scholar
  4. Clutton-Brock, T. H. (1974). Activity patterns of red colobus (Colobus badius tephrosceles). Folia Primatol. 21: 161-187.Google Scholar
  5. Dasilva, G. L. (1992). The western black-and-white colobus as a low-energy strategist: Activity budgets, energy expenditure and energy intake. J. Anim. Ecol. 61: 79-91.Google Scholar
  6. Defler, T. R. (1995). The time budget of a group of wild woolly monkeys (Lagothrix lagotricha). Int. J. Primatol. 16: 107-120.Google Scholar
  7. Fleagle J. (1999). Primate Adaptation and Evolution (2nd edn.), Academic Press, San Diego.Google Scholar
  8. Fowler, J., and Cohen, L. (1992). Practical Statistics for Field Biology, Wiley, Chichester, UK.Google Scholar
  9. Harrison, M. J. S. (1985). Time budget of the green monkey, Cercopithecus sabaeus: Some optimal strategies. Int. J. Primatol. 6: 351-376.Google Scholar
  10. Kurup, G. U., and Kumar, A. (1993). Time budget and activity patterns of the lion-tailed macaque (Macaca silenus). Int. J. Primatol. 14: 27-39.Google Scholar
  11. Lee, P. C. (1986). Environmental influences on development: Play, weaning and social structure. In Else, J. G., and Lee, P. C. (eds.), Primate Ontogeny, Cognition, and Social Behaviour, Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, UK, pp. 227-237.Google Scholar
  12. Li, Z. Y. (1992). Time budgets of Presbytis leucocephalus. Acta Theriol. Sin. 12: 7-13.Google Scholar
  13. Li, Z. Y. (2000). The Socioecology of White-Headed Langurs, Presbytis leucocephalus, and its Implications for Their Conservation, Unpublished PhD Thesis, The University of Edinburgh, Scotland.Google Scholar
  14. Li, Z. Y. (2002). Conservation action plan for white-headed langurs (Trachypithecus leucocephalus) in Guangxi, China. In Abstracts of the XIXth Congress of the IPS, Beijing, p. 68.Google Scholar
  15. Li, Z. Y., Wei, Y., and Rogers, M. E. (in press) Food choice of white-headed langurs in Fusui, China. Int. J. Primatol.Google Scholar
  16. Li, Z. Y., Liu, Z., Wei, Y., Rogers, E., and Hu, G. (2003). Population status of white-headed langur (Trachypithecus leucocephalus) in Guangxi, China. TigerPaper 30(2): 21-29.Google Scholar
  17. Malik, I. (1986). Time budgets and activity patterns in free-ranging rhesus monkey. In Else, J. G., and Lee, P. C. (eds.), Primate Ecology and Conservation, Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, UK, pp. 105-114.Google Scholar
  18. Marsh, C. W. (1978). Comparative activity budgets of red colobus. In Chivers, D. J., and Herbert, J. (eds.), Recent Advances in Primatology, Academic Press, London, pp. 250-251.Google Scholar
  19. Marsh, C. W. (1981). Time budget of Tana River red colobus. Folia Primatol. 35: 30-50.Google Scholar
  20. Martin, P. (1982). The energetic costs of play: Definition and estimation. Anim. Behav. 30: 294-295.Google Scholar
  21. Martin, P., and Bateson, P. (1993). Measuring Behaviour: An Introductory Guide, (2nd edn.), Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, UK.Google Scholar
  22. Menard, N., and Vallet, D. (1997). Behavioral responses of Barbary macaques (Macaca sylvanus) to variations in environmental conditions in Algeria. Am. J. Primatol. 43: 285-304.Google Scholar
  23. Menon, S., and Poirier, F. E. (1996). Lion-tailed macaques (Macaca silenus) in a disturbed forest fragment: activity patterns and time budget. Int. J. Primatol. 17: 969-985.Google Scholar
  24. Oates, J. F. (1977). The guereza and its food. In Clutton-Brock, T. H. (ed.), Primate Ecology: Studies of Feeding and Ranging Behaviour in Lemurs, Monkeys and Apes, Academic Press, London.Google Scholar
  25. Oates, J. F. (1987). Food distribution and foraging behavior. In Smuts, B. B., Seyfarth R. M., Cheney, D. L., Wrangham, R. W., and Struhsaker, T. T. (eds.), Primate Societies, The University of Chicago Press, Chicago, pp. 197-209.Google Scholar
  26. Poirier, F. E., Bellisari, A., and Haines, L. (1978). Functions of primate play behavior. In Smith, E. O. (ed.), Social Play in Primates, Academic Press, New York, pp. 143-168.Google Scholar
  27. Post, D. (1981). Activity patterns of yellow baboons (Papio cynocephalus) in the Amboseli National Park, Kenya. Anim. Behav 29: 357-374.Google Scholar
  28. Post, W., and Baulu, J. (1978). Time budgets of Macaca mulatta. Primates 19: 125-140.Google Scholar
  29. Sommer, V., and Mendoza-Granados, D. (1995). Play as indicator of habitat quality: A field study of langur monkeys (Presbytis entellus). Ethology 99: 177-192.Google Scholar
  30. Stanford, C. B. (1991). The Capped Langur in Bangladesh: Behavioral Ecology and Reproductive Tactics, S. Karger, Basel.Google Scholar
  31. Watanuki, Y., and Nakayama, Y. (1993). Age difference in activity pattern of Japanese monkeys: Effects of temperature, snow, and diet. Primates, 34: 419-430.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Plenum Publishing Corporation 2004

Authors and Affiliations

  • Zhaoyuan Li
    • 1
  • Elizabeth Rogers
    • 2
  1. 1.Faculty of Conservation BiologySouthwest Forestry CollegeBailongsi, Kunming, YunnanP.R. China
  2. 2.ICAPB, University of EdinburghEdinburghScotland

Personalised recommendations