Advertisement

Music as Embodied Mathematics: A Study of a Mutually Informing Affinity

  • Jeanne Bamberger
  • Andrea diSessa
Article

Abstract

The argument examined in this paper is that music – when approached through making and responding to coherent musical structures,facilitated by multiple, intuitively accessible representations – can become a learning context in which basic mathematical ideas can be elicited and perceived as relevant and important. Students' inquiry into the bases for their perceptions of musical coherence provides a path into the mathematics of ratio,proportion, fractions, and common multiples. Ina similar manner, we conjecture that other topics in mathematics – patterns of change,transformations and invariants – might also expose, illuminate and account for more general organizing structures in music. Drawing on experience with 11–12 year old students working in a software music/math environment, we illustrate the role of multiple representations, multi-media, and the use of multiple sensory modalities in eliciting and developing students' initially implicit knowledge of music and its inherent mathematics.

implicit knowledge mathematics in music multiple representations musical cognition 

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

REFERENCES

  1. Aristotle (c. 340). Physics: Book IV, Chapter 10, in The Works of Aristotle, Volume 1, Trans. W.D. Ross. Encyclopedia Britannica, Inc. Chicago: 1952.Google Scholar
  2. Arnon, I., Nesher, P. and Nirenburg, R. (2001). Where do fractions encounter their equivalents? International Journal of Computers for Mathematical Learning 6(2): 167-214.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Bach, J. S. (1781). The Goldberg Variations.Frankfurt: C.F. Peters.Google Scholar
  4. Bamberger, J. (2003). The development of intuitive musical understanding: A natural experiment. Psychology of Music 31(1): 7-36.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Bamberger, J. (1990). The laboratory for making things: Developing multiple representations of knowledge. In D. A. Schön (Ed.), The Reflective Turn. New York: Teachers College PressGoogle Scholar
  6. Bamberger, J. (1995). The Mind Behind the Musical Ear. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  7. Bamberger, J. (1996). Turning music theory on its ear: Do we hear what we see; do we see what we say? International Journal of Computers for Mathematical Learning 1(1): 33-55.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Bamberger, J. (1998). Action knowledge and symbolic knowledge: The computer as mediator. In D. Schön, B. Sanyal and W. Mitchel (Eds), High Technology and Low Income Communities (pp. 235-263). Cambridge: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  9. Bamberger, J. (2000). Developing Musical Intuitions: A Project-Based Introduction to Making and Understanding Music. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  10. Blum, D. (1986). The Art of Quartet Playing: The Guarneri Quartet in Conversation with David Blum.New York: Alfred A. Knopf.Google Scholar
  11. Confrey, J. and Smith, E. (1995). Splitting, covariation and their role in the development of exponential functions. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education 26(1): 66-86.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Dahlhaus, C. (1990). Studies on the Origin of Harmonic Tonality. Princeton: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
  13. diSessa, A. A. and Abelson, H. (1981). Turtle Geometry: The Computer as a Medium for Exploring Mathematics. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  14. diSessa, A. A. and Sherin, B. (1998). What changes in conceptual change? International Journal of Science Education 20(10): 1155-1191.Google Scholar
  15. Hasty, C. F. (1997).Meter as Rhythm. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  16. Haydn, F. J. (1773). Piano Sonata #25.Google Scholar
  17. Hofstadter, D. (1979). Gödel, Escher, Bach. New York: Basic Books, Inc.Google Scholar
  18. Lerdahl, F. and Jackendoff, R. (1983). A Generative Theory of Tonal Music. Cambridge, MA: MIT PressGoogle Scholar
  19. Leibnitz, J. G. Quoted in Miller, A. I. (2000). Insights of Genius (p. 192). Cambridge: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  20. Lewin, D. (1987). Generalized Musical Intervals and Transformations. New Haven: Yale University Press.Google Scholar
  21. Minsky, M. (1986). The Society of Mind. New York: Simon and Schuster.Google Scholar
  22. Rosner, B. S. and Meyer, L. B. (1982). Melodic processes and the perception of music. In D. Deutch (Ed.), The Psychology of Music (pp. 317-340). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  23. Rothstein, E. (1995). Emblems of Mind. New York: Times Books/Random House.Google Scholar
  24. Schoenberg, A. (1921). Suite for Piano, Op. 25. Vienna: Universal Edition.Google Scholar
  25. Tanay, D. (1999). Noting Music, Marking Culture: The Intellectual Context of Rhythmic Notation, 1250-1400. American Institute of Musicology; Holzgerlingen: Hanssler-Verlag.Google Scholar
  26. Thompson, P. W. (1996). Imagery and the development of mathematical reasoning. In L. P. Steffe, P. Nesher, P. Cobb, G. Goldin and B. Greer (Eds), Theories of Mathematical Learning (pp. 267-283). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
  27. Wagner, J. F. (2003). The construction of similarity. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. Graduate School of Education, University of California, Berkeley.Google Scholar
  28. Wilensky, U. and Resnick, M. (1999). Thinking in levels: A dynamic systems perspective to making sense of the world. Journal of Science Education and Technology 8(1): 3-19.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Kluwer Academic Publishers 2003

Authors and Affiliations

  • Jeanne Bamberger
    • 1
  • Andrea diSessa
    • 2
  1. 1.Music and Theater Arts, Buildin t 4-246Massachusetts Institute of TechnologyCambridgeUSA
  2. 2.University of CaliforniaBerkeleyUSA

Personalised recommendations