Advertisement

Cognitive Therapy and Research

, Volume 28, Issue 3, pp 415–432 | Cite as

Approach and Avoidance Goals and Plans: Their Relationship to Anxiety and Depression

  • Joanne M. DicksonEmail author
  • Andrew K. MacLeod
Article

Abstract

Numerous studies have identified cognitive and affective similarities and differences between anxiety and depression but relatively little empirical research has studied these emotional disturbances from a motivational perspective. This study investigates individual response-variations on approach and avoidance goals and plans. High anxiety (n = 27), high depression (n = 25), mixed (n = 30), and control (n = 30) adolescents selected from a larger school sample completed tasks that measured number and specificity of personal approach and avoidance goals and plans. High depression and mixed (depression, anxiety) adolescents generated fewer approach goals (but not more avoidance goals), fewer approach plans, more avoidance plans and were less specific in forming their goals and plans than controls. High anxiety adolescents generated more avoidance goals (but not fewer approach goals), more avoidance plans, fewer approach plans, and were less specific in forming approach but not avoidance goals and plans than controls. Overall the findings suggest that approach and avoidance motivational aspects are important in understanding anxiety and depression.

anxiety depression goals and plans 

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

REFERENCES

  1. Beck, A. T. (1976). Cognitive therapy and the emotional disorders. New York: International Universities Press.Google Scholar
  2. Beck, A. T., Epstein, N., Brown, G., & Steer, R. A. (1988). An inventory for measuring clinical anxiety: Psychometric properties. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 56, 893–897.Google Scholar
  3. Beck, A. T., & Steer, R. A. (1987). Beck Depression Inventory manual. San Antonio, Tx: Psychological Corporation Harcourt Brace Jovanovich.Google Scholar
  4. Beck, A. T., & Steer, R. A. (1993). Manual for the Beck Anxiety Inventory. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation Harcourt Brace.Google Scholar
  5. Beck, A. T., Steer, R. A., & Garbin, M. G. (1988). Psychometric properties of the Beck Depression Inventory: Twenty-five years of evaluation. Clinical Psychological Review, 8, 77–100.Google Scholar
  6. Burke, M., & Mathews, A. (1992). Autobiographical memory and clinical anxiety. Cognition and Emotion, 6, 23–35.Google Scholar
  7. Coats, E. J., Janoff-Bulman, R., & Alpert, N. (1996). Approach versus avoidance goals: Differences in self-evaluation and well-being. Personality and Social Bulletin, 22, 1057–1067.Google Scholar
  8. Dickson, J. M., & MacLeod, A. K. (2004). Anxiety, depression, and approach and avoidance goals. Cognition and Emotion, 18, 423–430.Google Scholar
  9. Dykman, B. M. (1998). Integrating cognitive and motivational factors in depression: Initial tests of a goal-orientation approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 139–158.Google Scholar
  10. Eccles, J. S., & Wigfield, A. (2002). Motivational beliefs, values, and goals. Annual Review of Psychology, 53, 109–132.Google Scholar
  11. Elliot, A. J., & Sheldon, K. M. (1998). Avoidance personal goals and the personality illness relationship. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 1282–1299.Google Scholar
  12. Elliot, A. J., Sheldon, K. M., & Church, M. A. (1997). Avoidance personal goals and subjective well-being. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 23, 915–927.Google Scholar
  13. Elliot, A. J., & Thrash, T. M. (2002). Approach-avoidance motivation in personality: Approach and avoidance temperament and goals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82, 804–818.Google Scholar
  14. Emmons, R. A. (1992). Abstract versus concrete goals: Personal striving level, physical illness, and psychological well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 62, 292–300.Google Scholar
  15. Emmons, R.A. (1996). Striving and feeling: Personal goals and subjective well-being. In P. M. Gollwitzer & J. A. Bargh (Eds.), The psychology of action: Linking cognition and motivation to behaviour (pp. 313–337). New York: Guilford.Google Scholar
  16. Fowles, D. C. (1988). Psychophysiology and psychopathology: A motivational approach. Psychophysiology, 25, 373–391.Google Scholar
  17. Fowles, D. C. (1994). A motivational theory of psychopathology. In W. D. Spaulding (Ed.), Integrative views of motivation, cognition, and emotion. Nebraska symposium on motivation (Vol. 41, pp. 181–238). London: University of Nebraska Press.Google Scholar
  18. Gotlib, I. H., & Cane, D. B. (1989). Self-report assessment of depression and anxiety. In P. C. Kendall & D. Watson (Eds.), Anxiety and depression: Distinctive and overlapping features (pp. 131–163). California: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  19. Gray, J. (1982). The neuropsychology of anxiety: An enquiry into the functions of the septo-hippocampal system. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  20. Hankin, B. L., Abramson, L.Y., Moffitt, T. E., Silva, P. A., McGee, R., & Angell, K. E. (1998). Development of depression from preadolescence to young adulthood: Emerging gender differences in a 10-year longitudinal study. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 107, 128–141.Google Scholar
  21. Kendall, P. C., Hollon, S. D., Beck, A. T., Hammen, C. L., & Ingram, R. E. (1987). Issues and recommendations regarding use of the Beck Depression Inventory. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 11, 289–300.Google Scholar
  22. Locke, E. A. (2002). Setting goals for life and happiness. In C.R. Snyder & S. J. Lopez (Eds.), Handbook of positive psychology (pp. 299–311). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  23. MacLeod, A. K., & Cropley, M. L. (1995). Depressive future-thinking: The role of valence and specificity. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 19, 35–50.Google Scholar
  24. MacLeod, A. K., Tata, P., Kentish, J., & Jacobsen, H. (1997). Retrospective and prospective cognitions in anxiety and depression. Cognition and Emotion, 11, 467–479.Google Scholar
  25. Mathews, A., & MacLeod, C. (1994). Cognitive approaches to emotion and emotional disorders. Annual Review of Psychology, 45, 25–50.Google Scholar
  26. McIntosh, W. D. (1996). When does goal non attainment lead to negative emotional reactions, and when doesn't it? The role of linking and rumination. In L. L. Martin & A. Tesser. (Eds.), Strivings and feelings (pp. 53–78). New Jersey: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  27. Mineka, S., Watson, D., & Clark, L. A. (1998). Comorbidity of anxiety and unipolar mood-disorders. Annual Review of Psychology, 49, 377–412.Google Scholar
  28. Moore, R. G., Watts, F. N., & Williams J. M. G. (1988). The specificity of personal memories in depression. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 27, 275–276.Google Scholar
  29. Nezlak, J. B. (2001). Daily psychological adjustment and the planfulness of day-to-day behaviour. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 20, 452–475.Google Scholar
  30. Oishi, S., Diener, E., Suh, E. M., & Lucas, R. E. (1999). Value as moderator in subjective well-being. Journal of Personality, 67, 157–184.Google Scholar
  31. Petersen, A. C., Compas, B. E., Brooks-Gunn, J., Stemmler, M., Ey, S., & Grant, K. E. (1993). Depression in adolescence. American Psychologist, 48, 155–168.Google Scholar
  32. Prenda, K. M., & Lachman, M. E. (2001). Planning for the future in life management strategy for increasing control and life satisfaction in adulthood. Psychology and Aging, 16, 206–216.Google Scholar
  33. Richards, A., & Whittaker, T. M. (1990). Effects of anxiety and mood manipulation in autobiographical memory. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 29, 145–154.Google Scholar
  34. Scholnick, E. K., & Friedman, S. L. (1993). Planning in context: Developmental and situational considerations. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 16, 145–167.Google Scholar
  35. Sheldon, K. M., Kasser, T., Smith, K., & Share, T. (2002). Personal goals and psychological growth: Testing an intervention to enhance goal attainment and personality integration. Journal of Personality, 70, 5–31.Google Scholar
  36. Taylor, E. B., & Klein, D. N. (1989). Assessing recovery in depression: Validity of symptom inventories. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 13, 1–8.Google Scholar
  37. Teasdale, J. D., & Barnard, P. J. (1993). Affect, cognition and change. London, UK: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  38. Williams, J. M. G. (1996). Depression and specificity of autobiographical memory. In D. Rubin (Ed.), Remembering our past: Studies in autobiographical memory (pp. 244–267). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  39. Williams, J. M. G., Ellis, N. C., Tyers, C., Healy, C., Rose, G. S., & MacLeod, A. K. (1996). The specificity of autobiographical memory and imageability of the future. Memory and Cognition, 24, 116–125.Google Scholar
  40. Williams, J. M. G., & Scott, J. (1988). Autobiographical memory in depression. Psychological Medicine, 18, 689–695.Google Scholar
  41. Wittchen, H. U., Nelson, C. B., & Lachner, G. (1998). Prevalence of mental disorders and psychosocial impairments in adolescents and young adults. Psychological Medicine, 28, 109–126.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Plenum Publishing Corporation 2004

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Royal HollowayUniversity of LondonLondonUnited Kingdom

Personalised recommendations