Advertisement

Biology and Philosophy

, Volume 19, Issue 1, pp 127–143 | Cite as

Ideas are not replicators but minds are

  • Liane Gabora
Article

Abstract

An idea is not a replicator because it does not consist of coded self-assembly instructions. It may retain structure as it passes from one individual to another, but does not replicate it. The cultural replicator is not an idea but an associatively-structured network of them that together form an internal model of the world, or worldview. A worldview is a primitive, uncoded replicator, like the autocatalytic sets of polymers widely believed to be the earliest form of life. Primitive replicators generate self-similar structure, but because the process happens in a piecemeal manner, through bottom-up interactions rather than a top-down code, they replicate with low fidelity, and acquired characteristics are inherited. Just as polymers catalyze reactions that generate other polymers, the retrieval of an item from memory can in turn trigger other items, thus cross-linking memories, ideas, and concepts into an integrated conceptual structure. Worldviews evolve idea by idea, largely through social exchange. An idea participates in the evolution of culture by revealing certain aspects of the worldview that generated it, thereby affecting the worldviews of those exposed to it. If an idea influences seemingly unrelated fields this does not mean that separate cultural lineages are contaminating one another, because it is worldviews, not ideas, that are the basic unit of cultural evolution.

acquired characteristics associative network autocatalytic closure conceptual closure culture evolution idea origin of life replicator self-replication worldview 

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Aunger R. (ed.) 2000. Darwinizing Culture: The Status of Memetics as a Science. Oxford University Press, Oxford.Google Scholar
  2. Baldassarre G. 2001. 'Cultural Evolution of 'Guiding Criteria' and Behaviour in a Population of Neural-network Agents'. Journal of Memetics 4.Google Scholar
  3. Blackmore S. 1999. The Meme Machine. Oxford University Press, Oxford.Google Scholar
  4. Blackmore S. 2000. 'The Power of Memes'. Scientific American 283: 52–61.Google Scholar
  5. Borger T. and Sarin R. 1997. 'Learning through Reinforcement and the Replicator Dynamics'. Journal of Economic Theory 77: 1–14.Google Scholar
  6. Borkar V.S., Jain S. and Rangarajan G. 1998. 'Dynamics of Individual Specialization and Global Diversification in Communities'. Complexity 3: 50–56.Google Scholar
  7. Boyd R. and Richerson P.J. 1985. Culture and the Evolutionary Process. The University of Chicago Press, Chicago.Google Scholar
  8. Campbell D. 1987. 'Evolutionary Epistomology'. In: Radnitzky G. and Bartley W.W. III (eds), Evolutionary Epistomology, Rationality, and the Sociology of Knowledge. Open Court, LaSalle, IL.Google Scholar
  9. Cavalli-Sforza L.L. and Feldman M.W. 1981. Cultural Transmission and Evolution: a Quantitative Approach. Princeton University Press, Princeton.Google Scholar
  10. Cemin S.C. and Smolin L. 1997. 'Coevolution of Membranes and Channels: A Possible Step in the Origin of Life'. Journal of Theoretical Biology (October issue) {adap-org/9709004}.Google Scholar
  11. Churchland P.S. and Sejnowski T. 1992. The Computational Brain. MIT Press, Cambridge.Google Scholar
  12. Corballis M.C. 2002. From Hand to Mouth: The Origins of Language. Princeton University Press, Princeton.Google Scholar
  13. Csanyi V. 1989. Evolutionary Systems and Society: A General Theory of Life, Mind and Culture. Duke University Press, Durham.Google Scholar
  14. Cziko G. 1997. Without Miracles: Universal Selection Theory and the Second Darwinian Revolution. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA.Google Scholar
  15. Dawkins R. 1976. The Selfish Gene. Oxford University Press, Oxford.Google Scholar
  16. Dawkins R. 1982. The Extended Phenotype. Oxford University Press, Oxford.Google Scholar
  17. Dennett D. 1995. Darwin's Dangerous Idea. Simon & Schuster, New York.Google Scholar
  18. Durham W.H. 1991. Coevolution: Genes, Culture, and Human Diversity. Stanford University Press, Stanford.Google Scholar
  19. Edelman G.M. 1987. Neural Darwinism: The Theory of Neuronal Group Selection. Basic Books, New York.Google Scholar
  20. Eigen M. and Schuster P. 1979. The Hypercycle: A Principle of Natural Self-organization. Springer, New York.Google Scholar
  21. Farmer J.D., Kauffman S.A. and Packard N.H. 1986. 'Autocatalytic Replication of Polymers'. Physica D 14.Google Scholar
  22. Farmer D. and Lo A.W. 1999. 'Frontiers of Finance: Evolution and Efficient Markets' Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA. 96:, pp. 9991–9992.Google Scholar
  23. Gabora L. 1995. 'Meme and variations: A Computer Model of Cultural Evolution'. In: Nadel L. and Stein D. (eds), 1993 Lectures in Complex Systems. Addison-Wesley, Boston.Google Scholar
  24. Gabora L. 1996. 'A Day in the Life of a Meme'. Philosophica 57: 901–938.Google Scholar
  25. Gabora L. 1997. 'The Origin and Evolution of Culture and Creativity'. Journal of Memetics 1: 1–27.Google Scholar
  26. Gabora L. 1998. 'Autocatalytic Closure in a Cognitive System: A Tentative Scenario for the Origin of Culture'. Psycholoquy 9 {adap-org /9901002}. http: / /www.cogsci.soton.ac.uk / cgi / psyc/ newpsy?9.67: 1–26.Google Scholar
  27. Gabora L. 1999. 'Weaving, Bending, Patching, Mending the Fabric of Reality: A Cognitive Science Perspective on Worldview Inconsistency'. Foundations of Science 3: 395–428.Google Scholar
  28. Gabora L. 2000. 'Conceptual Closure: Weaving Memories into an InterconnectedWorldview'. In: Van de Vijver G. and Chandler J. (eds), Closure: Emergent Organizations and their Dynamics: Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences Vol. 901., pp. 42–53.Google Scholar
  29. Gabora L. 2000. 'From Double Aspect Theory to Human Consciousness'. Consciousness and Cognition 9: S78.Google Scholar
  30. Gabora L. 2002. 'Amplifying Phenomenal Information: Toward a Fundamental Theory of Consciousness'. Journal of Consciousness Studies 9 {adap-org /9911003}: 3–29.Google Scholar
  31. Gabora L. 'Contextual focus: A cognitive explanation for the cultural revolution of the Middle/Upper Paleolithic'. (submitted).Google Scholar
  32. Gabora L. and Aerts D. 2002a. 'Contextualizing Concepts' Proceedings of the 15th International FLAIRS Conference (Special Track ''Categorization and Concept Representation: Models and Implications''). American Association for Artificial Intelligence, Pensacola, Florida. May 16-18, http: / /www.vub.ac.be/CLEA/ liane/ papers/ flairs.html., pp. 148–152.Google Scholar
  33. Gabora L. and Aerts D. 2002b. 'Contextualizing Concepts using a Mathematical Generalization of the Quantum Formalism'. Journal of Experimental and Theoretical Artificial Intelligence 14: 327–358.Google Scholar
  34. Gabora, L. and Deses, D. Transforming Memories into a Model of the World through Conceptual Closure, in prep.Google Scholar
  35. Godfrey-Smith P. 2000. 'The Replicator in Retrospect'. Biology and Philosophy 15: 403–423.Google Scholar
  36. Hancock P.J.B., Smith L.S. and Phillips W.A. 1991. 'A Biologically Supported Error-Correcting Learning Rule'. Neural Computation 3: 201–212.Google Scholar
  37. Hebb D.O. 1949. The Organization of Behavior. Wiley, New York.Google Scholar
  38. Heyes C.M. 1998. 'Theory of mind in nonhuman primates'. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 21: 104–134.Google Scholar
  39. Hinton G.E., McClelland J.L. and Rumelhart D.E. 1986. 'Distributed representations'. In: Rumelhart D.E., McClelland J.L. and the PDP research Group (eds), Parallel Distributed Processing. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, pp. 77–109.Google Scholar
  40. Hofbauer J. and Sigmund K. 1988a. The Theory of Evolution and Dynamical Systems. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, UK.Google Scholar
  41. Hull D.L. 1988a. Science as a Process: An Evolutionary Account of the Social and Conceptual Development of Science. University of Chicago Press, Chicago.Google Scholar
  42. Hull D.L. 1988b. 'Interactors versusVehicles'. In: Plotkin H.C. (ed.), The Role of Behavior in Evolution. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA.Google Scholar
  43. Hull D.L., Langman R.E. and Glenn S.S. 2001. 'A General Account of Selection: Biology, Immunology, and Behavior'. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 24: 511–573.Google Scholar
  44. Karmiloff-Smith A. 1990. 'Constraints on Representational Change: Evidence from Children's Drawing'. Cognition 34: 57–83.Google Scholar
  45. Karmiloff-Smith A. 1992. Beyond Modularity: A Developmental Perspective on Cognitive Science. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA.Google Scholar
  46. Kauffman S.A. 1993. Origins of Order. Oxford University Press, Oxford, UK.Google Scholar
  47. Kauffman S.A. 1999. 'Darwinism, neoDarwinism, and the autocatalytic model of culture: Commentary on Origin of Culture by Liane Gabora'. Psycoloquy 10 http: / /www.cogsci.soton.ac.uk/psyc-bin/ newpsy?10.022: 1–4.Google Scholar
  48. Kruschke J.K. 1993. 'Human category learning: Iimplication for backpropagation models'. Connection Science 5: 3–36.Google Scholar
  49. Lake M. 1998. 'Digging for Memes: The Role of Material Objects in Cultural Evolution'. In: Renfrew C. and Scarre C. (eds), Cognition and Material Culture: The Archeology of Symbolic Storage. McDonald Institute Monographs, pp. 77–88.Google Scholar
  50. Lee D.H., Granja J.R., Martinez J.A., Severin K. and Ghadiri M.R. 1996. 'A Self-Replicating Peptide'. Nature 382: 525–528.Google Scholar
  51. Lee D., Severin K., Yokobayashi M. and Ghadiri M. 1997. 'Emergence of Symbiosis in Peptide Self-replication through a Hypercyclic Network'. Nature 390: 591–594.Google Scholar
  52. Lewontin R.C. 1991. Biology as Ideology. Harper, New York.Google Scholar
  53. Lorenz K. 1971. Studies in Animal and Human Behavior. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA.Google Scholar
  54. Lumsden C. and Wilson E. 1981. Genes, Mind, and Culture: The Coevolutionary Process. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA.Google Scholar
  55. Marr D. 1969. 'A Theory of the Cerebellar Cortex'. Journal of Physiology 202: 437–470.Google Scholar
  56. Marsden P. 2001. 'Copycat Terrorism: Fanning the Fire'. Journal of Memetics 5.Google Scholar
  57. Metcalfe J.S. 2001. 'Consumption, Preferences, and the Evolutionary Agenda'. Journal of Evolutionary Economics 11: 37–58.Google Scholar
  58. Mithen S. 1998. 'A creative explosion? Theory of mind, language, and the disembodied mind of the Upper Paleolithic'. In: Mithen S. (ed.), Creativity in Human Evolution and Prehistory. Routledge.Google Scholar
  59. Morowitz H.J. 1992. The Beginnings of Cellular Life. Yale University Press, New Haven, CT.Google Scholar
  60. Nanay B. 2002. 'The Return of the Replicator: What is Philosophically Significant in a General Account of Replication and Selection?'. Biology and Philosophy 171: 9–121.Google Scholar
  61. Oparin A.I. 1971. 'Routes for the Origin of the First Forms of Life'. Subcellular and Cellular Biochemistry 1.Google Scholar
  62. Palm G. 1980. 'On associative memory'. Biological Cybernetics 36: 19–31.Google Scholar
  63. Piaget J. 1936/52. The Origin of Intelligence in Children. International Universities Press.Google Scholar
  64. Plotkin H. 1997. Darwin Machines and the Nature of Knowledge. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA.Google Scholar
  65. Popper K. 1963. Conjectures and Refutations. Routledge, London.Google Scholar
  66. Rivkin J.W. 2001. 'Reproducing Knowledge: Replication without Imitation at Moderate Complexity'. Organizational Science 12: 274–293.Google Scholar
  67. Saviotti P.P. and Mani G.S. 1995. 'Competition, Variety, and Technological Evolution: A Replicator Dynamics Model'. Journal of Evolutionary Economics 5: 369–392.Google Scholar
  68. Schuster P. and Sigmund K. 1983. Replicator Dynamics'. Journal of Theoretical Biology 100: 533–538.Google Scholar
  69. Severin K., Lee D.H., Kennan A.J. and Ghadiri M.R. 1997. 'A Synthetic Peptide Ligase'. Nature 389: 706–709.Google Scholar
  70. Sims C. 1991. Artificial Evolution for Computer Graphics. Computer Graphics 25: 325–327.Google Scholar
  71. Smolensky P. 1988. 'On the proper treatment of connectionism'. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11: 1–43.Google Scholar
  72. Sober E. and Wilson D.S. 1994. 'A Critical Review of Philosophical Work on the Units of Selection Problem'. Philosophy of Science 49: 157–180.Google Scholar
  73. Spector L. and Luke S. 1996a. 'Culture Enhances the Evolvability of Cognition'. In: Cottrell G. (ed.), Proceedings of the Eighteenth Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Mahwah, NJ, pp. 672–677.Google Scholar
  74. Spector L. and Luke S. 1996b. 'Cultural Transmission of Information in Genetic Programming'. In: Koza J.R., Goldberg D.E., Fogel D.B. and Riolo R.L. (eds), Genetic Programming 1996: Proceedings of the First Annual Conference. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, pp. 209–214.Google Scholar
  75. Sperber D. 1996. Explaining Culture: A Naturalistic Approach. Blackwell Publishers, Oxford.Google Scholar
  76. Sporns O., Gally J.A., Reeke G.N. Jr and Edelman G.M. 1989. 'Reentrant Signalling among Simulated Neuronal Groups leads to Coherency in their Oscillatory Activity' Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA. 86:, pp. 7265–7269.Google Scholar
  77. Sporns O., Tonini G. and Edelman G.M. 1991. 'Modeling Perceptual Grouping and Figure-Ground Segregation by Means of Active Reentrant Connections' Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA. 88:, pp. 129–133.Google Scholar
  78. Sterelny K., Smith K. and Dickison M. 1996. 'The Extended Replicator'. Biology and Philosophy 11: 377–403.Google Scholar
  79. Sterelny K. 2000. 'Looking after number one'. Biology and Philosophy 15: 275–289.Google Scholar
  80. Tomasello M. 2000. The Cultural Origins of Human Cognition. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA.Google Scholar
  81. Tonini G., Sporns O. and Edelman G.M. 1992. 'Reentry and the Problem of Integrating Multiple Cortical Areas: Simulation of Dynamic Integration in the Visual System'. Cerebellar Cortex 2: 310–335.Google Scholar
  82. Von Neumann J. 1966. Theory of Self-reproducing Automata. University of Illinois Press, Champaign.Google Scholar
  83. Witt U. 1992. 'Evolutionary Concepts in Economics'. Eastern Economic Journal 18: 405–419.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Kluwer Academic Publishers 2004

Authors and Affiliations

  • Liane Gabora
    • 1
  1. 1.Center Leo Apostel for Interdisciplinary Studies (CLEA)Free University of Brussels (VUB)BrusselsBelgium

Personalised recommendations