Advertisement

Journal of Adult Development

, Volume 8, Issue 2, pp 81–97 | Cite as

The Development of Implicit Epistemologies During Early and Middle Adulthood

  • Anna-Maija Pirttilä-Backman
  • Anna Kajanne
Article

Abstract

This study examined the development of implicit epistemologies of Finnish adults during the last ten years. It was hypothesized that specific adult experiences are stimulating for development. Moreover, a person's exploratory orientation was hypothesized to be related to development. Kitchener and King's model and method of Reflective Judgment provided tools to study adolescents' and adults' epistemologies and their possible development. Participants (N = 59) were given initial (1986–1988) and follow-up (1993–1994) Reflective Judgment interviews. The age range was from 24–50 years in the follow-up groups. The Reflective Judgment mean stage scores clearly increased during the two study periods (M1 = 4.64,M2 = 5.10; F(1, 58) = 32.82, p = .000). Education, in particular education beyond a person's primary profession/occupation, was a strong predictor of development. Also, encountering diversity and exploratory orientation were related to development, but their connections were more complicated. No gender differences were found. The results support the idea that positive changes in thinking and reasoning take place during adulthood.

adults development implicit epistemologies adulthood 

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

REFERENCES

  1. Alexander, C. N., &Langer, E. J. (Eds.). (1990). Higher stages of human development: Perspectives on adult growth. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  2. Armon, C. (1993). Developmental conceptions of good work: A longitudinal study. In J. Demick &P.M. Miller (Eds.), Development in the workplace (pp. 21-37). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  3. Armon, C. (1998). Adult moral development, experience, and education. Journal of Moral Education, 27, 345-371.Google Scholar
  4. Armon, C., &Dawson, T. L. (1997). Developmental trajectories in moral reasoning across the life span. Journal of Moral Education, 26, 433-453.Google Scholar
  5. Baxter Magolda, M. B. (1992). Knowing and reasoning in college: Gender-related patterns in students' intellectual development. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.Google Scholar
  6. Belenky, M. F.,Clinchy, B. M.,Goldberger, N. R., &Tarule, J. M. (1986). Women's ways of knowing. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
  7. Botella, L., &Gallifa, J. (1995). A constructivist approach to the development of personal epistemic assumptions and worldviews. Journal of Constructivist Psychology, 8, 1-18.Google Scholar
  8. Boyes, M. C. (1987). Implications of emerging doubt for adolescent identity formation. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. University of British Columbia.Google Scholar
  9. Boyes, M. C., &Chandler, M. (1992). Cognitive development, epistemic doubt and identity formation in adolescence. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 21, 277-304.Google Scholar
  10. Broughton, J. (1978). Development of concepts of self, mind, reality and knowledge. In W. Damon (Ed.), Social cognition: New directions for child development (pp. 75-100). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.Google Scholar
  11. Cacioppo, J. T., &Petty, R. E. (1982). The need for cognition. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 42, 116-131.Google Scholar
  12. Cacioppo, J. T.,Petty, R. E.,Feinstein, J. A., &Jarvis, W. B. G. (1996). Dispositional differences in cognitive motivation: the life and times of individuals varying in need for cognition. Psychological Bulletin, 119, 2, 197-253.Google Scholar
  13. Campbell, D., &Hansen, J. (1981). Manual fo the SVII-SCII: Strong-Campbell Interest Inventory. Palo Alto: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
  14. Carey, S., &Smith, C. (1993). On understanding the nature of scientific knowledge. Educational Psychologist, 28, 235-251.Google Scholar
  15. Chandler, M. (1987). The Othello effect. Essay on the emergence and eclipse of skeptical doubt. Humal Development, 30, 137-159.Google Scholar
  16. Colby, A., &Kohlberg L. (1987). The measurement of moral judgment. Vols. I & II. Standard issue scoring manual. New York: Cambrigde University Press.Google Scholar
  17. Commons, M. L.,Armon, C.,Richards, F. A.,Schrader D. E.,Farrell, E. W.,Tappan, M. B., &Bauer, N. F. (1989). A multidomain study of adult development. In M. L. Commons,J. Sinnot,F. A. Richards &C. Armon (Eds.), Adult development, Vol. 1, Comparisons and applications of adolescent and adult developmental models (pp. 33-56). New York: Praeger.Google Scholar
  18. Commons, M. L.,Richards, F. A., &Kuhn, D. (1982). Systematic and metasystematic reasoning: A case for a level of reasoning beyond Piaget's formal operations. Child Development, 53, 1058-1069.Google Scholar
  19. Commons, M. L.,Richards, F. A., &Armon, C. (Eds.). (1984). Beyond formal operations. Late Adolescent and Adult Cognitive Development. New York: Praeger.Google Scholar
  20. Commons, M. L.,Trudeau, E. J.,Stein, S. A.,Richards, F. A., &Krause, S. R. (1998). The existence of developmental stages as shown by the hierarchical complexity of tasks. Developmental Review, 8, 237-278.Google Scholar
  21. Edmondson, K. M., &Novak, J. D. (1993). The interplay of scientific epistemological views, learning strategies, and attitudes of college students. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 30, 547-559.Google Scholar
  22. Fischer, K. W. (1980). A theory of cognitive development: the control and construction of hierarchies of skills. Psychological Review, 87, 477-531.Google Scholar
  23. Fischer, K. W., &Silvern, L. (1985). Stages and individual differences in cognitive development. Annual Reviews of Psychology, 36, 613-648.Google Scholar
  24. Harvey, O. J.,Hunt, D. E., &Schroder, H. M. (1961). Conceptual systems and personality organization. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  25. Holland, J. L. (1973). Making vocational choices: A theory of careers. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.Google Scholar
  26. Jehng, J.-C.,Johnson, S. D., &Anderson, R. C. (1993). Schooling and students' epistemological beliefs about learning. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 18, 23-25.Google Scholar
  27. King, P. M. (1977). The development of reflective judgment and formal operational thinking in adolescents and young adults. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. University of Minnesota.Google Scholar
  28. King, P. M., &Kitchener, K. S. (1994). Developing reflective judgment. Understanding and promoting intellectual growth and critical thinking in adolescents and adults. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.Google Scholar
  29. Kitchener, K. S. (1978). Intellectual development in late adolescents and young adults: Reflective Judgment and verbal reasoning. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. University of Minnesota.Google Scholar
  30. Kitchener, K. S., &King, P. M. (1981). Reflective Judgment: Concepts of justification and their relationship to age and education. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 2, 89-116.Google Scholar
  31. Kitchener, K. S., &King, P. M. (1985). Reflective Judgment scoring rules. Revised edition. Available from the authors.Google Scholar
  32. Kitchener, K. S., &King, P. M. (1990). The Reflective Judgment Model: Ten years of research. In M. L. Commons,J. D. Sinnot,F. A. Richards &C. Armon (Eds.), Adult development, Vol. 2, Models and methods in the study of adolescent and adult thought (pp. 63-79). New York: Praeger.Google Scholar
  33. Kitchener, K. S.,Lynch, C. L.,Fischer, K. W., &Wood, P. K. (1993). Developmental range of reflective judgment: the effect of contextual support and practice on developmental stage. Developmental Psychology, 29, 893-906.Google Scholar
  34. Kitchener, K. S., &Wood, P. K. (1987). Development of concepts of justification in German university students. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 10, 171-185.Google Scholar
  35. Kohlberg, L. (1969). Stage and sequence: The cognitive-developmental approach to socialization. In D. A. Goslin (Ed.), Handbook of socialization theory and research (pp. 347-480). Chicago: Rand McNally.Google Scholar
  36. Kohlberg, L., &Armon, C. (1984). Three types of stage models used in the study of adult development. In M. L. Commons,F. A. Richards, & Armon, C. (Eds.), Beyond formal operations. Late adolescent and adult cognitive development (pp. 383-394). New York: Praeger.Google Scholar
  37. Loevinger, J. (with the assistance of A. Blasi) (1976). Ego development. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.Google Scholar
  38. Loevinger, J., &Wessler, R. (1970). Measuring ego development. Vol. 1, San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.Google Scholar
  39. Martin, J. E.,Silva, D. G.,Newman, J. H., &Thayer, J. F. (1994). An investigation into the structure of epistemological style. Personality and Individual Differences, 16, 617-629.Google Scholar
  40. Miller, M. E. (1994). World views, ego development, and epistemological changes from the conventional to the postformal: a longitudinal study. In M. E. Miller &S. R. Cook-Greuter (Eds.), Transcendence and mature thought in adulthood (pp. 147-179). Lantham: Rowman & Littlefield.Google Scholar
  41. Perry, W. G., Jr. (1970). Forms of intellectual and ethical development in the college years. A scheme. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.Google Scholar
  42. Pirttilä-Backman, A.-M. (1991). “Knowledge” and “The experts knowledge”: should there be a difference? Working papers 2/ 1991. Department of Social Psychology, University of Helsinki, Finland.Google Scholar
  43. Pirttilä-Backman, A.-M. (1993). The social psychology of knowledge reassessed. Toward a new delineation of the field with empirical substantiation. Annales Academiae Scientiarum Fennicae. Dissertationes Humanarum Litterarum 68. Helsinki, Finland: Academia Scientiarum Fennica.Google Scholar
  44. Richards, F. A., &Commons, M. L. (1984). Systematic, metasystematic, and cross-paradigmatic reasoning: a case for stages of reasoning beyond formal operations. In M. L. Commons,F. A. Richards, &C. Armon (Eds.), Beyond formal operations. Late adolescent and adult cognitive development (pp. 92-119). New York: Praeger.Google Scholar
  45. Rosenberg, M. (1965). Society and adolescent self-image. Princeton: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
  46. Rotter, J. B. (1966). Generalized expectancies for internal versus external control of reinforcement. Psychological Monographs, 80 (1).Google Scholar
  47. Schaie, K. W. (1996). Intellectual development in adulthood. The Seattle longitudinal study. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  48. Schommer, M. (1993). Epistemological development and academic performance among secondary students. Journal of Educational Psychology, 85, 406-411.Google Scholar
  49. Tinsley, H. E. A., &Weiss, D. J. (1975). Interrater reliability and agreement of subjective judgments. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 22, 358-376.Google Scholar
  50. United Nations Development Programme (UNDP). (1995). HumanDevelopment Report 1995. Recognizing women's contribution. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  51. Valecha, G. K. (1972). Construct validation of internal-external locus of control as measured by an abbreviated 11-item IE scale. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. The Ohio State University.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Plenum Publishing Corporation 2001

Authors and Affiliations

  • Anna-Maija Pirttilä-Backman
    • 1
  • Anna Kajanne
    • 1
  1. 1.Department of Social PsychologyUniversity of HelsinkiFinland

Personalised recommendations