Advertisement

Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders

, Volume 28, Issue 6, pp 467–478 | Cite as

Attentional Capacities in Children with Autism: Is There a General Deficit in Shifting Focus?

  • Daisy M. Pascualvaca
  • Bryan D. Fantie
  • Maria Papageorgiou
  • Allan F. Mirsky
Article

Abstract

Twenty-three children with autism and two control groups completed an attention battery comprising three versions of the continuous performance test (CPT), a digit cancellation task, the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST), and two novel, computerized tests of shifting attention (i.e., the Same–Different Computerized Task and the Computerized Matching Task). Children with autism could focus on a particular stimulus and sustain this focus as indicated by their performance on the digit cancellation task and the CPT. Their performance on the WCST suggested problems in some aspects of shifting attention (i.e., disengaging attention). The autism group performed as well as controls on the Same–Different Computerized Task, however, that required successive comparisons between stimuli. This implies that they could, in fact, shift their attention continuously. In addition, they did not differ from controls on the Computerized Matching Task, an analog of the WCST, suggesting that they do not have a general deficit in shifting attention.

Attention deficit autism shifting focus 

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

REFERENCES

  1. American Psychiatric Association. (1987). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (3rd ed., Rev.). Washington, DC: Author.Google Scholar
  2. Battig, K., Rosvold, H. E., & Mishkin, M. (1960). Comparison of the effects of frontal and caudate lesions on delayed response and alternation in monkeys. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 53, 400-404.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  3. Bellack, A. S., Mueser, K. T., Morrison, R. L., Tierney, A., & Podell, K. (1990). Remediation of cognitive deficits in schizophrenia. American Journal of Psychiatry, 147, 1650-1655.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  4. Braverman, M., Fein, D., Lucci, D., & Waterhouse, L. (1989). Affect comprehension in children with pervasive developmental disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 19, 301-316.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  5. Burack, J. A. (1994). Selective attention deficits in persons with autism: Preliminary evidence of an inefficient attentional lens. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 103, 535-543.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  6. Casey, B. J., Gordon, C. T., Mannheim, G. B., & Rumsey, J. M. (1993). Dysfunctional attention in autistic savants. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 15, 933-946.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  7. Courchesne, E. (1995). Infantile autism. Part 1: MR imaging abnormalities and their neurobehavioral correlates. International Pediatrics, 10, 141-154.Google Scholar
  8. Courchesne, E., Akshoomoff, N. A., Townsend, J., & Saitoh, O. (1995). A model system for the study of attention and the cerebellum: Infantile autism. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology Supplement, 44, 315-325.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  9. Courchesne, E., Townsend, J., Akshoomoff, N. A., Saitoh, O., Yeung-Courchesne, R., Lincoln, A. J., James, H. E., Haas, R. H., Schreibman, L., & Lau, L. (1994a). Impairment in shifting attention in autistic and cerebellar patients. Behavioral Neuroscience, 108, 848-65.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  10. Courchesne, E., Townsend, J., Akshoomoff, N. A., Yeung-Courchesne, R., Press, G. A., Murakami, J. W., Lincoln, A. J., James, H. E., Saitoh, O., Eggas, B., Haas, R. H., & Schreibman, L. (1994b). A new finding: impairment in shifting attention in autistic and cerebellar patients. In S. H. Broman & J. Grafinan (Eds.), Atypical cognitive deficits in developmental disorders: Implications for brain function (pp. 101-137). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  11. Davies, S., Bishop, D., Manstead, A. S., & Tantam, D. (1994). Face perception in children with autism and Asperger's syndrome. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines, 35, 1033-1057.Google Scholar
  12. Dawson, G., & Lewy, A. (1989). Arousal, attention, and the socioemotional impairments of individuals with autism. In G. Dawson (Ed.), Autism: Nature, diagnosis, and treatment (pp. 49-74). New York: Guilford.Google Scholar
  13. Fein, D., Lucci, D., Braverman, M., & Waterhouse, L. (1992). Comprehension of affect in context in children with pervasive developmental disorders. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines, 33, 1157-1167.Google Scholar
  14. Garretson, H. B., Fein, D., & Waterhouse, L. (1990). Sustained attention in children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 20, 101-114.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  15. Green, M. F., Ganzell, S., Satz, P., & Vaclav, J. F. (1990). Teaching the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test to schizophrenic patients (letter). Archives of General Psychiatry, 47, 91-92.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  16. Green, M. F., Satz, P., Ganzell, S., & Vaclav, J. F. (1992). Wisconsin Card Sorting Test performance in schizophrenia: Remediation of a stubborn deficit. American Journal of Psychiatry, 149, 62-67.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  17. Halperin, J. M., Wolf, L. E., Pascualvaca, D. M., Newcorn, J. H., Healey, J. M., O'Brien, J. D., Morganstein, A., & Young, J. G. (1988). Differential assessment of attention and impulsivity in children. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 27, 326-329.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  18. Halperin, J. M., Sharma, V., Greenblatt, E., & Schwartz, S. T. (1991). Assessment of the continuous performance test: Reliability and validity in a nonreferred sample. Psychological Assessment 3, 603-608.Google Scholar
  19. Heaton, R. K. (1981). The Wisconsin Card Sorting Test. Odessa: Psychological Assessment Resources.Google Scholar
  20. Hutt, S. J., Hutt, C., Lee, D., & Ounsted, C. (1964). Arousal and childhood autism. Nature, 204, 908-909.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  21. Kanner, L. (1943). Autistic disturbances of affective contact. Nervous Child, 2, 217-250.Google Scholar
  22. Kanner, L. (1944). Early infantile autism. Journal of Pediatrics, 25, 211-217.Google Scholar
  23. Lifshitz, M., Kugelmass, S., & Karov, M. (1985). Perceptual-motor and memory performance of high-risk children. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 11, 74-84.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  24. Lincoln, A. J., Dickstein, P., Courchesne, E., Elmasian, R., & Tallal, P. (1992). Auditory processing abilities in non-retarded adolescents and young adults with developmental receptive language disorder and autism. Brain and Language, 43, 613-622.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  25. Lovaas, O. (1977). The autistic child: Language development through behavior modification. New York: Irvington.Google Scholar
  26. Lovaas, O. (1993). The development of a treatment-research project for developmentally disabled and autistic children. Journal of Applied Behavioral Analysis, 26, 617-630.Google Scholar
  27. Lovaas, O., Koegel, R., & Schreibman, L. (1979). Stimulus overselectivity in autism: A review of research. Psychological Bulletin, 86, 1236-1254.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  28. Mirsky, A. F. (1987). Behavioral and psychophysiological markers of disordered attention. Environmental Health Perspectives, 74, 191-199.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  29. Mirsky, A. F., Anthony, B. J., Duncan, C. C., Ahearn, M. B., & Kellam, S. G. (1991). Analysis of the elements of attention: A neuropsychological approach. Neuropsychology Review, 2, 109-145.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  30. Mirsky, A. F., Fantie, B. D., & Tatman, J. E. (1995). Assessment of attention across the lifespan. In R. L. Mapou & J. Spector (Eds.), Clinical neuropsychological assessment: A cognitive approach (pp. 17-48). New York: Plenum Press.Google Scholar
  31. Ozonoff, S. (1995). Reliability and validity of the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test in studies of autism. Neuropsychology, 9, 491-500.Google Scholar
  32. Ozonoff, S., & McEvoy, R. E. (1994). A longitudinal study of executive function and theory of mind development in autism. Development and Psychopathology, 6, 415-431.Google Scholar
  33. Ozonoff, S., Pennington, B. F., & Rogers, S. J. (1991). Executive function deficits in high-functioning autistic individuals: Relationship to theory of mind. Journal of Child Psychology, Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines, 32, 1081-1105.Google Scholar
  34. Prior, M., & Hoffmann, W. (1990). Brief report: Neuropsychological testing of autistic children through an exploration with frontal lobe tests. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 20, 581-590.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  35. Rimland, B. (1964). Infantile autism. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.Google Scholar
  36. Rosvold, H., Mirsky, A. F., Saranson, I., Bransome, E. D., & Beck, L. H. (1956). A continuous performance test of brain damage. Journal of Consulting Psychology, 20, 343-350.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  37. Rumsey, J. M. (1985). Conceptual problem-solving in highly verbal, nonretarded autistic men. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 15, 23-36.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  38. Rumsey, J. M., & Hamburger, S. D. (1990). Neuropsychological divergence of high-level autism and severe dyslexia. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 20, 159-186.Google Scholar
  39. Schopler, E., Reichler, R. J., & Rochen-Renner, B. R. (1988). The Childhood Autism Rating Scale. Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services.Google Scholar
  40. Wechsler, D. (1991). Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children (3rd ed.). New York: Psychological Corp.Google Scholar
  41. Woodcock, R. W., & Johnson, M. B. (1990). Woodcock-Johnson Psycho-Educational Battery—Revised. Texas: DLM Teaching Resources.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Plenum Publishing Corporation 1998

Authors and Affiliations

  • Daisy M. Pascualvaca
    • 1
  • Bryan D. Fantie
    • 1
    • 2
  • Maria Papageorgiou
    • 1
  • Allan F. Mirsky
    • 1
  1. 1.Section on Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, Laboratory of Brain and CognitionNational Institute of Mental HealthBethesda
  2. 2.Human Neuropsychology Laboratory, Department of PsychologyThe American UniversityWashington, DC

Personalised recommendations