Skip to main content
Log in

The Responses of Autistic Children to the Distress of Others

  • Published:
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The behavior of preschool children from five groups (developmental language disordered, high-functioning autistic, low-functioning autistic, mentally retarded, and normally developing) were coded in three situations: presentation of a nonsocial orienting stimulus (an unfamiliar noise) and two social situations involving simulated distress on the part of an adult with whom they were playing. Cognitive level was correlated with level of responsiveness to stimuli only for the two retarded groups (mentally retarded and low-functioning autistic). Girls showed more prosocial behavior than boys in both social situations, independent of diagnosis. The language-disordered children showed only mild and subtle social deficits. The low-functioning autistic children showed pronounced deficits in responding in all situations. The mentally retarded and high-functioning autistic children showed good awareness of all situations, but were moderately impaired in their ability to respond prosocially; they rarely initiated prosocial behavior, but did respond to specific prompts. The behavioral feature that marked both autistic groups, in contrast to all other groups, was a lack of social referencing; they did not tend to look toward an adult in the presence of an ambiguous and unfamiliar stimulus. Results are discussed in terms of variability between and among high- and low-functioning autistic children, and implications for the core deficits in autism.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

REFERENCES

  • American Psychiatric Association. (1987). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. (3rd ed., rev.). Washington, D.C.: Author.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bachevalier, J. (1994). The contribution of medial temporal lobe structures in infantile autism: A neurobehavioral study in primates. In M. Bauman & T. L. Kemper (Eds.), The neurobiology of autism (pp. 146–169). Baltimore: Johns Hopkins Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S. (1991). Precursors to a theory of mind: Understanding attention in others. In A. Whiten (Ed.), Natural theories of mind. Evolution, development, and simulation of everyday mindreading. Oxford, U.K.: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bauman, M., & Kemper, T. L. (1994) Neuroanatomic observations of the brain in autism. In M. Bauman & T. L. Kemper (Eds.), The neurobiology of autism (pp. 119–145). Baltimore: Johns Hopkins Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bayley, N. (1969). Bayley Scales of Infant Development. Berkeley, CA: The Psychological Corp.

    Google Scholar 

  • Braverman, M., Fein, D., Lucci, D., & Waterhouse, L. (1989). Affect comprehension in children with pervasive developmental disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 19, 301–316.

    Google Scholar 

  • Capps, L., Kasari, C., Yirmiya, N., & Sigman, M. (1993). Parental perception of emotional expressiveness in children with autism. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 61, 475–484.

    Google Scholar 

  • Capps, L., Sigman, M., & Mundy, P. (1994) Attachment security in children with autism. Development and Psychopathology, 6, 249–261.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, D. J., Paul, R., & Volkmar, F. (1987). Issues in the classification of PDD and associated conditions. In D. J. Cohen, A. M. Donnellan, & R. R. Paul (Eds.), Handbook of autism and pervasive developmental disorders (pp. 20–40). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dunn, L. M., & Dunn, L. (1981). Peabody Picture Vocabulary—Revised. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eisenberg, N., Pasternack, J. F., Cameron, E., & Tryon, K. (1984). The relation of quantity and mode of prosocial behavior to moral cognition and social style. Child Development, 55, 1479–1485.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eisenberg-Berg, N., & Lennon, R. (1980). Altruism and the assessment of empathy in the preschool years. Child Development, 51, 552–557.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fein, D., Lucci, D., Braverman, M., & Waterhouse, L. (1992). Comprehension of affect in context in children with pervasive developmental disorders. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 33, 1157–1167.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fein, D., Pennington, B., Markowitz, P., Braverman, M., & Waterhouse, L. (1986). Towards a neuropsychological model of infantile autism: Are the social deficits primary? Journal of the American Academy of Child Psychiatry, 25, 198–212.

    Google Scholar 

  • Feinman, S. (1982) Social referencing in infancy. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 28, 445–470.

    Google Scholar 

  • Garfin, D. G., & Lord, C. (1986). Communication as a social problem in autism. In E. Schopler & G. B. Mesibov (Eds.), Social behavior and autism (pp. 103–131). New York: Plenum Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hauck, M., Fein, D., Waterhouse, L., & Feinstein, C. (1995). Social initiations by autistic children to adults and other children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 25, 579–595.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hobson, R. P. (1983). The autistic child's recognition of age-related features of people, animals and things. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 1, 343–352.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hobson, R. P. (1984). Early childhood autism and the question of egocentrism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 14, 85–104.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hobson, R. P. (1986a). The autistic child's appraisal of expressions of emotion. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 27, 321–342.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hobson, R. P. (1986b). The autistic child's appraisal of expressions of emotion: A further study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 27, 679–680.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hobson, R. P. (1989). On sharing experiences. Development and Psychopathology, 1, 197–203.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hendrick, D. L., Prather, E. M., & Tobin, A. R. (1984). Sequenced inventory of communication development (rev. ed.). Seattle: University of Washington Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Howlin, P. (1986). An overview of social behavior in autism. In E. Schopter & G. B. Mesibov (Eds.), Social Behavior and Autism (pp. 103–131). New York: Plenum Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hresko, W. P., Reid, D. K., & Hamill, D. D. (1981). The test of early language development. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kasari, C., Sigman, M., Baumgartner, P., & Stipek, D. (1993). Pride and mastery in children with autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 34, 353–362.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kasari, C., Sigman, M., & Yirmiya, N. (1993). Focused and social attention of autistic children in interactions with familiar and unfamiliar adults: A comparison of autistic, mentally retarded, and normal children. Development and Psychopathology, 5, 403–414.

    Google Scholar 

  • Klinnert, M., Campos, J., Sorce, J., Emde, R., & Svejda, M. (1983). Emotions as behavior regulators: Social referencing in infancy. In R. Plutchik & H. Kellerman (Eds.), Emotions in early development: Vol. 2. The emotions. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lord, C. (1993). The complexity of social behavior in autism. In S. Baron-Cohen, H. Tager-Flusberg, & D. Cohen (Eds.), Understanding other minds: Perspectives from autism. Oxford, U.K.: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lord, C. (1984). Development of peer relations in children with autism. In F. Morrison, C. Lord, & D. Keating (Eds.), Allied developmental Psychology (Vol. 1). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mchale, S. (1983). Social interactions of autistic and non-handicapped children during free play. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 53, 81–91.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mchale, S., Olley, J., Marcus, L., & Simeonssen, R. (1981). Nonhandicapped peers as tutors for autistic children. Exceptional Children, 48, 263–265.

    Google Scholar 

  • Moore, C., & Corkum, V. (1994). Social understanding at the end of the first year of life. Developmental Review, 14, 349–372.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mundy, P., & Sigman, M. (1989). Theoretical implications of joint attention deficits in autism. Development and Psychopathology, 1, 173–183.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mundy, P., Sigman, M., & Kasari, C. (1994). Joint attention, developmental level, and symptom presentation in autism. Development and Psychopathology, 6, 389–401.

    Google Scholar 

  • Radke-Yarrow, M., & Zahn-Waxler, C. (1976). Dimensions and correlates of prosocial behavior in young children. Child Development, 47, 118–125.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rapin, I. (Ed.). (1996). Preschool children with inadequate communication: Developmental language disorder, autism, low IQ. In Clinics in Developmental Medicine (No. 139). London: Mac Keith.

  • Rheingold, H. L., Hay, D. F., & West, M. J. (1976). Sharing in the second year of life. Child Development, 47, 1148–1158.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reuter, J., Stancin, T., & Craig, P. (1981). Kent scoring adaptation of the Bayley Scales of Infant Development. Kent, OH: Kent Developmental Metrics.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ross, H. S., & Goldman, B. D. (1977). Establishing new social relationships in infancy. In T. Alloway, P. Pliner, & L. Krames (Eds.), Advances in the study of communication and affect: Vol. 3. Attachment behavior (pp. 61–79). New York: Plenum Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rutter, M. (1984). Infantile autism and other pervasive developmental disorders. In M. Rutter and L. Hersov (Eds.), Child and Adolescent Psychiatry: Modern Approaches (Vol 2). Oxford, U.K.: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rutter, M., & Garmezy, N. (1983). Developmental psychopathology. In E. M. Hetherington (Ed.), Handbook of child psychology (Vol. 4). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sigman, M. D., Ungerer, J., Mundy, P., & Sherman, T. (1984). Cognitive functioning in autistic children. In D. Cohen, A. Donnellan, & R. Paul (Eds.), Handbook of autism and atypical Development. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sigman, M. D., Kasari, C., Kwon, J. H., & Yirmiya, N. (1992). Responses to the negative emotions of others by autistic, mentally retarded, and normal children. Child Development, 63, 796–807.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sparrow, S. A., Balla, D. A., & Cicchetti, D. V. (1984). Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.

    Google Scholar 

  • Staub, E. (1970). A child in distress: The influence of age and number of witnesses on children's attempts to help. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 14, 130–140.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thorndike, R. L., Hagen, E. P., & Sattler, J. M. (1986). Stanford-Binet Intelligence Scale (4th ed.). Chicago: Riverside.

    Google Scholar 

  • Trevarthen, C. (1979). Communication and cooperation in early infancy: A description of primary intersubjectivity. In M. Bullowa (Ed.), Before speech: The beginnings of interpersonal communication. Cambridge, U.K.: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Trevarthen, C., and Hubley, P. (1978). Secondary Intersubjectivity: Confidence, confiding, and acts of meaning in the first year. In A. Lock (Ed.), Action, gesture, and symbol: The emergence of language. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tsai, L. (1992). Diagnostic issues in high-functioning autism. In E. Schopler & G. Mesibov (Eds.), High-functioning individuals with autism. New York: Plenum Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ungerer, J., & Sigman, M. (1981). Symbolic play and language comprehension in autistic children. Journal of the American Academy of Child Psychiatry, 20, 318–337.

    Google Scholar 

  • Volkmar, F. R., & Klin, A. (1993). Social development in autism: Historical and clinical perspectives. In S. Baron-Cohen, H. Tager-Flusberg, & D. Cohen (Eds.), Understanding other minds: Perspectives from autism. Oxford, U.K.: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wing, L. (1980). Early childhood autism. Oxford, U.K.: Pergamon.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yarrow, M. R., Scott, P. M., & Waxler, C. Z. (1973). Learning concern for others. Developmental Psychology, 8, 240–260.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yirmiya, N., Sigman, M., Kasari, C., & Mundy, P. (1992). Empathy and cognition in high-functioning children with autism. Child Development, 63, 150–160.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zahn-Waxler, C., & Radke-Yarrow, M. (1990). The origins of empathic concern. Motivation and Emotion, 14, 107–130.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zahn-Waxler, C., Radke-Yarrow, M., Wagner, E., & Chapman, M. (1992). Development of concern for others. Developmental Psychology, 28, 126–136.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zahn-Waxler, C., Robinson, J. L., & Emde, R. (1992). The development of empathy in twins. Developmental Psychology, 28, 1038–1047.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Bacon, A.L., Fein, D., Morris, R. et al. The Responses of Autistic Children to the Distress of Others. J Autism Dev Disord 28, 129–142 (1998). https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1026040615628

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1026040615628

Navigation