Advertisement

Reading and Writing

, Volume 16, Issue 7, pp 693–716 | Cite as

Analogy without priming in early spelling development

  • Marie-Line Bosse
  • Sylviane Valdois
  • Marie-Josèphe Tainturier
Article

Abstract

This article reports three experimentsinvestigating the use of analogies in spellingacquisition. French children spelledpseudowords to dictation, some of which werephonological neighbours of words with uncommonendings (e.g., /daby/ derived from ``début''/deby/). A more frequent use of these uncommongraphemes in neighbour pseudowords than incontrol pseudowords was taken as evidence forspelling by analogy. In Experiment 1, ananalogy effect was observed in Grades 3 to 5.Younger children did not use analogies, butthey were also unable to spell most referencewords. Experiments 2 and 3 introduced areference word learning phase prior to thepseudoword dictation task. An analogy effectwas found in second graders (Experiment 2) andeven in first graders (Experiment 3) whenchildren knew how to spell most referencewords. Comparable use of analogies was observedin children with comparable lexical knowledgeindependently of their grade level oralphabetic skills. The results suggest thatchildren establish specific orthographicknowledge from the beginning of literacyacquisition and use this knowledge to generatenew word spellings as soon as it isavailable.

Acquisition Analogy Children French Learning Neighbourhood Pseudoword Spelling Writing 

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Ans, B., Carbonnel, S. & Valdois, S. (1998). A connectionist multiple-trace memory model for polysyllabic word reading. Psychological Review 105, 678-723.Google Scholar
  2. Barry, C. (1988). Modelling assembled spelling: Convergence of data from normal subjects and "surface" dysgraphia. Cortex 24, 339-346.Google Scholar
  3. Bernstein, S.E. & Treiman, R. (2001). Learning a novel grapheme: Effects of positional and phonemic context on children's spelling. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 79, 56-77.Google Scholar
  4. Brown, A. (1990). A review of recent research in spelling. Educational Psychology Review 2, 365-397.Google Scholar
  5. Brown, G.D.A. & Loosemore, R.P.W. (1994). Computational approaches to normal and impaired spelling. In G.D.A. Brown & N.C. Ellis (eds.), Handbook of spelling: Theory, process and intervention (pp. 319-335). Chichester, UK: John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
  6. Campbell, R. (1985). When children write pseudowords to dictation. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 57, 26-41.Google Scholar
  7. Content, A., Mousty, P. & Radeau, M. (1990). BRULEX: Une base de données lexicales informatisée pour le fran çais écrit et parlé (BRULEX: A computerised lexical database for spoken and written French). L'Année Psychologique (the Psychological Year) 90, 551-566.Google Scholar
  8. Deavers, R.P. & Brown, G.D.A. (1997). Rules versus analogies in children's spelling: Evidence for task dependence. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal 9, 339-361.Google Scholar
  9. Dixon, M. & Kaminska, Z. (1994). Casting a spell with witches and broomsticks: Direct and associative influences on pseudoword orthography. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology 6, 383-398.Google Scholar
  10. Ehri, L.C. (1986). Sources of difficulty in learning to spell and read. In M.L. Wolraich & D. Routh (eds.), Advances in developmental and behavioral pediatrics, vol. 7 (pp. 121-195). Greenwich, CT: JAI.Google Scholar
  11. Ehri, L.C. & Saltmarsh, J. (1995). Beginning readers outperform older disabled readers in learning to read words by sight. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal 7, 295-326.Google Scholar
  12. Frith, U. (1985). Beneath the surface of developmental dyslexia. In K.E. Patterson, J.C. Marshall & M. Coltheart (eds.), Surface dyslexia (pp. 301-330). London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.Google Scholar
  13. Gentry, J.R. (1982). An analysis of developmental spelling in GNYS AT WRK. Reading Teacher 36, 192-200.Google Scholar
  14. Goswami, U. (1988). Children's use of analogy in learning to spell. British Journal of Developmental Psychology 6, 21-33.Google Scholar
  15. Houghton, G. & Zorzi, M. (2003). Normal and impaired spelling in a connectionist dual-route architecture. Cognitive Neuropsychology 20, 115-162.Google Scholar
  16. Kreiner, D.S. (1992). Reaction time measures of spelling: Testing a two-strategy model of skilled spelling. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition 18, 765-776.Google Scholar
  17. Lennox, C. & Siegel, L.S. (1994). The role of phonological and orthographic processes in learning to spell. In G.D.A. Brown & N.C. Ellis (eds.), Handbook of spelling: Theory, process and intervention (pp. 93-109). Chichester, UK: John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
  18. Manis, F.R. (1985). Acquisition of word identification skills in normal and disabled readers. Journal of Educational Psychology 77, 78-90.Google Scholar
  19. Marsh, G., Friedman, M., Welch, V. & Desberg, P. (1980). The development of strategies in spelling. In U. Frith (ed.), Cognitive processes in spelling (pp. 339-354). London: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  20. Muter, V., Snowling, M.J. & Taylor, S. (1994). Orthographic analogies and phonological awareness: Their role and significance in early reading development. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 35, 293-310.Google Scholar
  21. Nation, K. & Hulme, C. (1996). The automatic activation of sound-letter knowledge: An alternative interpretation of analogy and priming effects in early spelling development. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 63, 416-435.Google Scholar
  22. Nation, K. & Hulme, C. (1998). The role of analogy in early spelling development. In C. Hulme & R.M. Joshi (eds.), Reading and spelling: Development and disorders (pp. 433-445). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
  23. Nunes, T., Bryant, P. & Bindman, M. (1997). Morphological spelling strategies: Developmental stages and processes. Developmental Psychology 33, 637-649.Google Scholar
  24. Pacton, S., Fayol, M. & Perruchet, P. (2001). The acquisition of untaught orthographic regularities in French. In L. Verhoeven, C. Erlbro & P. Reitsma (eds.), Precursors of functional literacy. Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Kluwer Academic Publishers.Google Scholar
  25. Perfetti, C.A. (1992). The representation problem in reading acquisition. In P. Gough, L.C. Ehri & R. Treiman (eds.), Reading acquisition (pp. 145-174). Hillsdale, New Jersey: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  26. Plaut, D., McClelland, J., Seidenberg, M. & Patterson, K. (1996). Understanding normal and impaired word reading: Computational principles in quasi-regular domains. Psychological Review 103, 56-115.Google Scholar
  27. Rapp, B., Epstein, C. & Tainturier, M.J. (2002). The integration of information across lexical and sublexical processes in spelling. Cognitive Neuropsychology 19, 1-29.Google Scholar
  28. Reitsma, P. (1983). Printed word learning in beginning readers. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 36, 321-339.Google Scholar
  29. Rittle-Johnson, B. & Siegler, R.S. (1999). Learning to spell: Variability, choice and change in children's strategy use. Child Development 70, 332-348.Google Scholar
  30. Share, D.L. (1995). Phonological recoding and self-teaching: Sine qua non of reading acquisition. Cognition 55, 151-218.Google Scholar
  31. Share, D.L. (1999). Phonological recoding and orthographic learning: A direct test of the self-teaching hypothesis. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 72, 95-129.Google Scholar
  32. Siegler, R.S. (1995). How does change occur: A microgenetic study of number conservation. Cognitive Psychology 28, 225-273.Google Scholar
  33. Snowling, M.J. (1994). Towards a model of spelling acquisition: The development of some component skills. In G.D.A. Brown & N.C. Ellis (eds.), Handbook of spelling: Theory, process and intervention (pp. 111-128). Chichester, UK: John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
  34. Stuart, M. & Coltheart, M. (1988). Does reading develop in a sequence of stages? Cognition 50, 139-181.Google Scholar
  35. Tainturier, M.J. & Rapp, B. (2001). The spelling process. In B. Rapp (ed.), What deficits reveal about the human mind: A handbook of cognitive neuropsychology. Philadelphia: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
  36. Tainturier, M.J., Bosse, M.L., Valdois, S. & Rapp, B. (2000). Lexical neighbourhood effects in nonword spelling. Presented at the Annual Meeting of the Psychonomic Society, New Orleans, November.Google Scholar
  37. Templeton, S. & Bear, D. (1992). Development of orthographic knowledge and the foundations of literacy. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
  38. Treiman, R. (1998). Beginning to spell in English. In C. Hulme & R.M. Joshi (eds.), Reading and spelling: Development and disorders (pp. 371-393). Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
  39. Varnhagen, C.K., Boechler, P.M. & Steffler, D.J. (1999). Phonological and orthographic influences on children's vowel spelling. Scientific Studies of Reading 3, 363-379.Google Scholar
  40. Varnhagen, C.K., McCallum, M. & Burstow, M. (1997). Is children's spelling naturally stage-like? Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal 9, 451-481.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Kluwer Academic Publishers 2003

Authors and Affiliations

  • Marie-Line Bosse
    • 1
  • Sylviane Valdois
    • 1
  • Marie-Josèphe Tainturier
    • 2
  1. 1.Laboratoire de Psychologie et Neuro-Cognition (UMR 5105 CNRS)Université Pierre Mendès FranceGrenobleFrance
  2. 2.Marie-Line Bosse or Sylviane Valdois, Laboratoire de Psychologie et Neuro-CognitionUniversité Pierre Mendés FranceGrenoble CedexFrance

Personalised recommendations