Advertisement

Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology

, Volume 25, Issue 3, pp 209–215 | Cite as

Levels of Analysis in Cognitive Bases of Maternal Disciplinary Dysfunction

  • Zvi Strassberg
Article

Abstract

This study tested alternative hypotheses concerning relations between mothers' disciplinary dysfunction and their descriptive versus inference-level interpretations of child noncompliance. Mothers of aggressive boys (MAGGs; n = 19) and mothers of average boys (MAVGs; n = 17) were presented with hypothetical vignettes of compliance situations (mean ages; mothers = 26.8 years, children = 4.5 years). Each vignette ended with the child being compliant or with each of a variety of noncompliant behaviors (request, statement, complaint, ignore, or oppose). Dependent variables were mothers' judgments of noncompliance severity (a descriptive measure), and attributions of defiant intent to the child (an inferential measure). Findings across analyses consistently pointed to attributions as more discriminating than judgments in differentiating between maternal groups. It was concluded that models of maternal discipline dysfunction should focus on analysis of inferential rather than descriptive cognitive responses to child noncompliance, and that parenting interventions should incorporate attribution-training into treatment protocols.

Discipline behavior problems attributions 

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

REFERENCES

  1. Achenbach, T., & Edelbrock, C. (1981). Behavior problems and competencies reported by parents of normal and disturbed children aged four through sixteen. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 46(1, Serial No. 188).Google Scholar
  2. Achenbach, T. M., & Edelbrock, C. (1986). Manual for the Teacher's Report Form and Teacher Version of the Child Behavior Profile. Burlington: University of Vermont, Department of Psychiatry.Google Scholar
  3. Berkowitz, L. (1989). Frustration-aggression hypothesis: Examination and reformulation. Psychological Bulletin, 106, 59–73.Google Scholar
  4. Bruner, J. S., Goodnow, J., & Austin, G. (1956). A study of thinking. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  5. Bugental, D. B., Mantyla, S. M., & Lewis, J. (1989). Parental attributions as moderators of affective communication to children at risk for physical abuse. In D. Cicchetti & V. Carlson (Eds.), Child Maltreatment: Therapy and Research on the Causes and Consequences of Child Abuse and Neglect (pp. 254–279). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  6. Bugental, D. B., Blue, J., & Lewis, J. (1990). Caregiver cognitions as moderators of affective reactions to difficult children. Developmental Psychology, 26, 631–638.Google Scholar
  7. Bugental, D. B., Blue, J., Cortez, V.,, Fleck, K., Kopeikin, H., Lewis, J. C., & Lyon, J. (1993). Social cognitions as organizers of autonomic and affective responses to social challenge. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64, 94–103.Google Scholar
  8. Dix, T. (1991). The affective organization of parenting: Adaptive and maladaptive processes. Psychological Bulletin, 110, 3–25.Google Scholar
  9. Dix, T., & Lochman, J. E. (1990). social cognition and negative reactions to children: A comparison of mothers of aggressive and nonaggressive boys. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 9, 418–438.Google Scholar
  10. Kenziora, K. T. (1995). Behavior tracking skills of mothers of problem and nonproblem toddlers. Unpublished manuscript, State University of New York at Stony Brook.Google Scholar
  11. Lorber, R., Reid, J. B., & Simard, K. (1982). Maternal tracking of childhood behaviors as mediated by family stress. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the American Psychological Association, Los Angeles.Google Scholar
  12. MacKinnon, C. E., Lamb, M. B., Belsky, J., & Baum, C. (1990). An affective-cognitive model of mother-child aggression. Development and Psychopathology, 2, 1–13.Google Scholar
  13. MacKinnon-Lewis, C., Volling, B. I., Lamb, M. E., Dechman, K., Rabiner, D., & Curtner, M. E. (1994). A cross-contextual analysis of boys' social competence from family to school. Developmental Psychology, 30, 325–333.Google Scholar
  14. Smith, A. M., & O'Leary, S. G. (1995). Attributions and arousal as predictors of maternal discipline. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 19, 345–357.Google Scholar
  15. Strassberg, Z. (1995). Social information processing in compliance situations by the mothers of behavior-problem boys. Child Development, 66, 376–389.Google Scholar
  16. Straus, M. A. (1979). Measuring intrafamily conflict and violence: The Conflict Tactics Scale. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 41, 75–88.Google Scholar
  17. Straus, M. A. (1987). Measuring physical and emotional abuse of children with the Conflict Tactics Scale. Unpublished report, Family Violence Research Program of the Family Research Laboratory, University of New Hampshire, Durham.Google Scholar
  18. Wahler, R. G., & Sansbury, L. E. (1990). The monitoring skills of troubled mothers: Their problem in defining child deviance. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 18, 577–589.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Plenum Publishing Corporation 1997

Authors and Affiliations

  • Zvi Strassberg
    • 1
  1. 1.Department of PsychologyThe State University of New YorkStony Brook

Personalised recommendations