Journal of Nonverbal Behavior

, Volume 21, Issue 1, pp 45–55 | Cite as

Testosterone, Smiling, and Facial Appearance

Abstract

In a study of possible links between testosterone and dominance, 119 men and 114 women provided saliva samples for testosterone assay and posed smiling and not smiling for portrait photographs. Expert judges viewing the photographs found smaller smiles among high than low testosterone men, with less zygomatic major (raising the corners of the mouth) and orbicularis oculi (raising the cheeks and crinkling around the corners of the eyes) muscle activity. Naive judges viewing individual photographs gave higher potency ratings to smiling high testosterone men than smiling low testosterone men. Naive judges viewing photographs grouped into high and low testosterone sets gave higher potency and lower goodness ratings to high than to low testosterone men, regardless of whether they were smiling. Among women, judges found only slight relationships between testosterone and facial appearance. The pattern among men of less smiling with higher testosterone levels fits with research linking testosterone to face-to-face dominance.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Albert, D. J., Walsh, M. L., & Jonik, R. H. (1993). Aggression in humans: What is its biological foundation? Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 17, 405–425.Google Scholar
  2. Archer, J. (1991). The influence of testosterone on human aggression. British Journal of Psychology, 82, 1–28.Google Scholar
  3. Bernhardt, P. C., Dabbs, J. M., Jr., Fielden, J. A., & Lutter, C. (1996). Testosterone changes during vicarious experiences of victory and defeat in spectators of sporting events. Manuscript under editorial review.Google Scholar
  4. Bernstein, I. S., Rose, R. M., & Gordon, T. P. (1974). Behavioral and environmental events influencing primate testosterone levels. Journal of Human Evolution, 3, 517–525.Google Scholar
  5. Booth, A., & Dabbs, J. M., Jr. (1993). Testosterone and men's marriages. Social Forces, 72, 463–477.Google Scholar
  6. Booth, A., Shelley, G., Mazur, A., Tharp, G., & Kittok, R. (1989). Testosterone, and winning and losing in human competition. Hormones and Behavior, 23, 555–571.Google Scholar
  7. Cashdan, E. (1995). Hormones, sex, and status in women. Hormones and Behavior, 29, 354–366.Google Scholar
  8. Dabbs, J. M., Jr. (1990). Salivary testosterone measurements: Reliability across hours, days, and weeks. Physiology and Behavior, 48, 83–86.Google Scholar
  9. Dabbs, J. M., Jr. (1993). Salivary testosterone measurements in behavioral studies. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 694, 177–183.Google Scholar
  10. Dabbs, J. M., Jr., Carr, T. S., Frady, R. L., & Riad, J. K. (1995). Testosterone, crime, and misbehavior among 692 male prison inmates. Personality and Individual Differences, 18, 627–633.Google Scholar
  11. Dabbs, J. M., Jr., de La Rue, D., & Williams, P. M. (1990). Testosterone and occupational choice: Actors, ministers, and other men. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59, 1261–1265.Google Scholar
  12. Dabbs, J. M., Jr., Hargrove, M. F., & Heusel, C. (1996). Testosterone differences among college fraternities: Well-behaved vs. rambunctious. Personality and Individual Differences, 20, 157–161.Google Scholar
  13. Dabbs, J. M., Jr., & Morris, R. (1990). Testosterone, social class, and antisocial behavior in a sample of 4,462 men. Psychological Science, 1, 209–211.Google Scholar
  14. Dabbs, J. M., Jr., Ruback, R. B., Frady, R. L., Hopper, C. H., & Sgoutas, D. S. (1988). Saliva testosterone and criminal violence among women. Personality and Individual Differences, 9, 269–275.Google Scholar
  15. Ekman, P., Davidson, R., & Friesen, W. V. (1990). Emotional expression and brain physiology II: The Duchenne smile. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58, 342–353.Google Scholar
  16. Ekman, P., & Friesen, W. V. (1978). Facial action coding system: A technique for the measurement of facial movement. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.Google Scholar
  17. Ekman, P., & Heider, K. G. (1988). The universality of a contempt expression: A replication. Motivation and Emotion, 12, 303–308.Google Scholar
  18. Exline, R. V. (1972). Visual interaction: The glances of power and preference. In J. K. Cole (Ed.), Nebraska Symposium on Motivation: Vol. 19. Current theory and research in motivation, (pp. 163–206). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.Google Scholar
  19. Friesen, W. (1986). Recent developments in FACS—EMFACS. Face Value: Facial Measurement Newsletter, 1(1), 1–1.Google Scholar
  20. Hall, J. A. (1984). Nonverbal sex differences: Communication accuracy and expressive style. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press.Google Scholar
  21. Hall, J. A., & Halberstadt, A. G. (1986). Smiling and gazing. In J. S. Hyde & M. C. Linn (Eds.), The psychology of gender: Advances through meta-analysis (pp. 136–158). Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press.Google Scholar
  22. Hess, U., Banse, R., & Kappas, A. (1995). The intensity of facial expression is determined by underlying affective state and social stimuli. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 280–288.Google Scholar
  23. LaFrance, M., & Carmen, B. (1980). The nonverbal display of psychological androgyny. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 38, 36–49.Google Scholar
  24. Lykken, D., & Tellegen, A. (1996). Happiness is a stochastic phenomenon. Psychological Science, 7, 186–189.Google Scholar
  25. Mazur, A. (1985). A biosocial model of status in face-to-face primate groups. Social Forces, 64, 377–402.Google Scholar
  26. Mazur, A., Mazur, J., & Keating, C. (1984). Military rank attainment of a West Point class: Effects of cadets' physical features. American Journal of Sociology, 90, 125–150.Google Scholar
  27. Mazur, A., & Mueller, U. (1996). Channel modeling: From West Point cadet to general. Public Administration Review, 56, 191–198.Google Scholar
  28. Meikle, A. W., Stringham, J. D., Bishop, D. T., & West, D. W. (1988). Quantitating genetic and nongenetic factors influencing androgen production and clearance rates in men. Journal of Clinical Endocrinology and Metabolism, 67, 104–109.Google Scholar
  29. Navarro, M. A., Juan, L., & Bonnin, M. R. (1986). Salivary testosterone: Relationship to total and free testosterone in serum. Clinical Chemistry, 32, 231–232.Google Scholar
  30. Osgood, C. E., Suci, G. J., & Tannenbaum, P. H. (1957). The measurement of meaning. Urbana, IL: University of Illinois.Google Scholar
  31. Schindler, G. L. (1979). Testosterone concentration, personality patterns, and occupational choice in women. Dissertation Abstracts International, 40, 1411A. (University Microfilms No. 79-19,403).Google Scholar
  32. Seaford, H. W., Jr. (1976). Cultural facial expression. In E. Giles & J. S. Friedlaender (Eds.), The measures of man (pp. 616–639). Cambridge, MA: Peabody Museum.Google Scholar
  33. Sherwin, B. B., Gelfand, M. M., & Brender, W. (1985). Androgen enhances sexual motivation in females: A prospective, crossover study of sex steroid administration in the surgical menopause. Psychosomatic Medicine, 47, 339–351.Google Scholar
  34. Vermeulen, A., & Verdonck, G. (1992). Representativeness of a single point plasma testosterone level for the long term hormonal milieu in men. Journal of Clinical Endocrinology and Metabolism, 74, 939–942.Google Scholar
  35. Zebrowitz, L. A., Olson, K., & Hoffman, K. (1993). Stability of babyfaceness and attractiveness across the life span. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64, 453–466.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Human Sciences Press, Inc. 1997

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of PsychologyGeorgia State UniversityAtlanta

Personalised recommendations