Narrative Ability in High-Functioning Children with Autism or Asperger's Syndrome

Article

Abstract

This study examines the narrative abilities of 28 high-functioning children with autism or Asperger's Syndrome and 22 typically developing children across two different discourse contexts. As compared with the typically developing children, the high-functioning group performed relatively well in the storybook context but exhibited difficulty imbuing their narratives of personal experience with the more sophisticated characteristics typically employed by the comparison group. Furthermore, children with autism or Asperger's Syndrome demonstrated impairments inferring and building on the underlying causal relationships both within and across story episodes in both narrative contexts. Findings further revealed that the narrative abilities of children with autism or Asperger's Syndrome were associated with performance on measures of emotional understanding, but not theory of mind or verbal IQ. Findings are discussed in relation to the social and emotional underpinnings of narrative discourse.

Autism Asperger's Syndrome narrative emotional understanding 

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. American Psychiatric Association (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: Author.Google Scholar
  2. Appelbee, A. N. (1978). The Child's concept of story. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
  3. Astington, J. (1990). Narrative and the child's theory of mind. In B. K. Britton & A. D. Pellegrini (Eds.), Narrative thought and narrative language (pp. 151-171). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  4. Bamberg, M. (1991). Narrative activity as perspective taking: The role of emotionals, negations, and voice in the construction of the story realm. Journal of Cognitive Psychotherapy, 5, 275-290.Google Scholar
  5. Bamberg, M. (1997). Positioning between structure and performance. Journal of Narrative & Life History. Special Issue: Oral Versions of Personal Experience: Three Decades of Narrative Analysis, 7, 335-342.Google Scholar
  6. Bamberg, M., & Damrad-Frye, R. (1991). On the ability to provide evaluative comments: Further explorations of children's narrative competencies. Journal of Child Language, 18, 689-710.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  7. Bamberg, M., & Marchman, V. (1991). Binding and unfolding: Towards the linguistic construction of narrative discourse. Discourse Processes, 14, 277-305.Google Scholar
  8. Bamberg, M., & Reilly, J. (1996). Emotion, narrative and affect: How children discover the relationship between what to say and how to say it. In D. Slobin, J. Gerhardt, A. Kyratzis, & J. Guo (Eds.), Interaction social context and language: Essays in honor of Susan Eryin-Tripp (pp. 329-341). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  9. Baron-Cohen, S. Leslie, A., & Frith, U. (1985). Does the autistic child have a “theory of mind?” Cognition, 21, 37-46.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  10. Berman, R., & Slobin, D. (1994). Relating events in narrative: A cross-linguistic developmental study. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  11. Bloom, L. (1991). Language development from two to three. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  12. Bruner, J. S. (1986). Actual minds, possible worlds. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  13. Bruner, J. S. (1990). Acts of meaning. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  14. Bruner, J. S., & Feldman, C. (1993). Theories of mind and the problem of autism. In S. Baron-Cohen, H. Tager-Flusberg, & D. J. Cohen (Eds.), Understanding other minds: Perspectives from autism. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  15. Capps, L., Ebeling, R., & Rasco, L. (1997). The Berkeley Empathy Measure. Unpublished manuscript, University of California at Berkeley.Google Scholar
  16. Capps, L., Kehres, J., & Sigman, M. (1998). Conversational abilities among children with autism and developmental delay. Autism, 2, 325-344.Google Scholar
  17. Capps, L., Losh, M., & Thurber, C. (2000). “The frog ate a bug and made his mouth sad”: Narrative competence in children with autism. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 28, 193-204.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  18. Capps, L., & Ochs, E. (1995). Constructing panic: The discourse of agoraphobia. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  19. Clark, P., & Rutter, M. (1981). Autistic children's responses to structure and to interpersonal demands. Journal of Autism and Developmental Syndromes, 11, 201-217.Google Scholar
  20. Goffman, E. (1959). The presentation of self in everday life. Garden City, NY: Doubleday.Google Scholar
  21. Goffman, E. (1974). Frame analysis: An essay on the organization of experience. New York: Harper & Row.Google Scholar
  22. Gopnik, A., Capps, L., & Meltzoff, A. (2000). Early theories of mind: What the theory theory can tell us about autism. In S. Baron-Cohen, H. Tager-Flusberg, D. Cohen (Eds.), Understanding other minds: Perspectives from developmental cognitive neuroscience, 2nd ed. (pp. 50-72). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  23. Grandin, T. (1995). Thinking in pictures. New York: Doubleday.Google Scholar
  24. Happé, F. (1994). An advanced test of theory of mind: Understanding of story characters' thoughts and feelings by able autistic, mentally handicapped, and normal children and adults. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 24, 129-154.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  25. Harré, R. (Ed.)(1986). The social construction of emotions. Oxford: Basil Blackwell.Google Scholar
  26. Hood, L., & Bloom, L. (1979). What, when, and how about why: Longitudinal study of early expressions of causality. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 44, 6.Google Scholar
  27. Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1985). Language and cognitive processes from a developmental perspective. Language and Cognitive Processes, 1, 61-85.Google Scholar
  28. Kasari, C., Sigman, M., & Yirmiya, N. (1993). Focused and social attention of autistic children in interactions with familiar and unfamiliar adults: A comparison of autistic, mentally retarded, and normal children. Development and Psychopathology, 5, 403-414.Google Scholar
  29. Kuntay, A. (1997). Extended discourse skills of turkish preschool children across shifting contexts. Unpublished doctoral dissertation: University of California, Berkeley.Google Scholar
  30. Labov, W. (1972). Language in the inner city. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania Press.Google Scholar
  31. Labov, W., & Waletzky, J. (1967). Narrative analysis: Oral versions of personal experience. In J. Helm (Ed.), Essays on the verbal and visual arts (pp. 12-44). Seattle: University of Washington Press.Google Scholar
  32. Lord, C., Rutter, M., & Le Couteur, A. (1994). Autism Diagnostic Interview—Revised: A revised version of a diagnostic interview for caregivers of individuals with possible pervasive developmental syndromes. Journal of Autism & Developmental Syndromes, 24, 659-685.Google Scholar
  33. Loveland, K., McEvoy, R., Tunali, B., & Kelley, M. L. (1990). Narrative story-telling in autism and Down syndrome. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 8, 9-23.Google Scholar
  34. Loveland, K., & Tunali, B. (1993). Narrative language in autism and the theory of mind hypothesis: A wider perspective. In S. Baron-Cohen, H. Tager-Flusberg, & D. Cohen (Eds.), Understanding other minds: Perspectives from autism (pp. 247-266). NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  35. Mandler, J. M. (1984). Stories, scripts, and scenes: Aspects of schema theory. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  36. Mayer, M. (1969). Frog, where are you? New York: Dial Press.Google Scholar
  37. McCabe, A., & Peterson, C. (1985). A naturalistic study of the production of causal connectives by children. Journal of Child Language, 12, 145-159.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  38. Miller, P., Hoogstra, L., Mintz, J., & Fung, H. (1993). Troubles in the graden and how they get resolved: A young child's transformations of his favorite story. In C. Nelson (Ed.), Memory and affect in development (pp. 87-114). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  39. Miller, P., & Sperry, L. L. (1987). The socialization of anger and aggression. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 33, 1-31.Google Scholar
  40. Miranda, A., Camp, L., Hemphill, L., & Wolf, D. (1992). Developmental changes in children's use of tense in narrative. Presented at the Boston University Conference on Language Development, Boston, MA.Google Scholar
  41. Nelson, K. (1986). Event knowledge: Structure and function in development. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  42. Nelson, K. (1989). Narratives from the crib. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  43. Ochs, E., & Capps, L. (1996). Narrating the self. Annual Review of Anthropology, 25, 19-43.Google Scholar
  44. Ochs, E., & Capps, L. (1997). Narrative authenticity. Journal of Narrative and Life History, 7, 83-89.Google Scholar
  45. Ochs, E., & Capps, L. (2001). Living narrative: creating lives in everyday storytelling. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  46. Perner, J., & Wimmer, H. (1985). “John thinks that Mary thinks that... ”: attribution of second-order beliefs by 5-to 10-yearold children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 39, 437-471.Google Scholar
  47. Peterson, C., & McCabe, A. (1983). Developmental psycholinguistics: Three ways of looking at a child's narrative. New York: Plenum.Google Scholar
  48. Polanyi, M. (1966). The Tacit Dimension. New York: Doubleday.Google Scholar
  49. Polkinghorne, D. (1988). Narrative knowing and the human sciences. Albany: State University of New York Press.Google Scholar
  50. Propp, V. (1968). The morphology of the folktale. T. Scott (Trans.), Austin: University of Texas Press.Google Scholar
  51. Reilly, J. (1992). How to tell a good story: The intersection of language and affect in children's narratives. Journal of Narrative and Life History, 2, 355-377.Google Scholar
  52. Reilly, J., Bates, E., & Marchman, V. (1998). Narrative discourse in children with early focal brain injury. Brain and Language, 61, 335-375.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  53. Reilly, J. S., Klima, E. S., & Bellugi, U. (1990). Once more with feeling: Affect and language in atypical populations. Development and Psychopathology, 2, 367-391.Google Scholar
  54. Ricoeur, P. (1988). Time and Narrative. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
  55. Sarbin, T. (1989). Emotions as narrative emplotments. In M. Packer, & R. Addison (Eds.), Entering the circle: Hermeneutic investigation in psychology. (pp. 185-201). Albany, NY: State University of New York Press.Google Scholar
  56. Shweder, R. (1993). The cultural psychology of the emotions. In M. Lewis and J. Haviland (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (pp. 417-431). New York: Guilford.Google Scholar
  57. Sobel, D., Capps, L., Losh, M., & Gopnik, A. (2000). Understanding the development of explanations in young children with autism. Presented at the International Conference for Infancy Studies, Brighton, UK.Google Scholar
  58. Stein, N., & Glenn, C. (1979). An analysis of story comprehension in elementary school children. In R. Freedle (Ed.), New directions in discourse processing (pp. 53-120). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
  59. Tager-Flusberg, H. (1992). Autistic children's talk about psychological states: Deficits in early acquisition of theory of mind. Child Development, 63, 161-172.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  60. Tager-Flusberg, H. (1995). 'Once upon a ribbit': Stories narrated by autistic children. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 13, 45-59.Google Scholar
  61. Tager-Flusberg, H., & Sullivan, K. (1995). Attributing mental states to story characters: A comparison on narratives produced by autistic and mentally retarded individuals. Applied Psycholinguistics, 16, 241-256.Google Scholar
  62. World Health Organization (1993). International classification of diseases, 10th ed. Chapter V. Mental and behavioral disorders (including disorders of psychological development). Geneva, Switzerland: Author.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Plenum Publishing Corporation 2003

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.University of California at BerkeleyBerkeley

Personalised recommendations