Cognitive Therapy and Research

, Volume 27, Issue 3, pp 309–330 | Cite as

Dysphoric Rumination Impairs Concentration on Academic Tasks

Article

Abstract

Three studies investigated the effects of dysphoric rumination on concentration during 3 academic tasks—reading a passage from the GRE (Study 1), watching a videotaped lecture (Study 2), and proofreading written text (Study 3). Before performing these tasks, dysphoric and nondysphoric students were induced either to ruminate about themselves or to distract themselves by focusing on neutral images (all three studies) or by planning an event (Study 1). The results supported our hypothesis that dysphoric rumination, relative to distraction, would impair students' concentration. In all 3 studies, dysphoric ruminators reported difficulty concentrating, as well as interfering thoughts, during the relevant academic tasks. Furthermore, dysphoric ruminators were slower than dysphoric distractors in completing the tasks—specifically, reading the GRE passage (Study 1) and answering lecture comprehension questions (Study 2). In addition, dysphoric participants who ruminated showed impaired reading strategies (Study 1), reduced comprehension of academic material (Study 2), and poor proofreading performance (Study 3). These findings suggest that, in addition to its documented adverse effects on mood, thinking, and problem-solving, self-focused rumination interferes with instrumental behavior. Implications for social relationships and job performance are discussed.

rumination self-focus distraction dysphopia depression concentration 

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (Rev. 4th ed.). Washington, DC: AuthorGoogle Scholar
  2. Baddeley, A. D., & Hitch, G. J. (1994). Developments in the concept of working memory. Neuropsychology, 8, 485–493.Google Scholar
  3. Beck, A. T. (1967). Depression: Clinical, experimental, and theoretical aspects. New York: Harper & Row.Google Scholar
  4. Brockner, J. (1979). The effects of self-esteem, success, failure, and self-consciousness on task performance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 1732–1741.Google Scholar
  5. Brockner, J., & Hulton, B. (1978). How to reverse the vicious cycle of low self-esteem: The importance of attentional focus. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 14, 564–578.Google Scholar
  6. Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (1990). Origins and functions of positive and negative affect: A control–process review. Psychology Review, 97, 19–35.Google Scholar
  7. Coddington, R. D., & Troxell, J. R. (1980). The effect of emotional factors on football injury rates—A pilot study. Journal of Human Stress, 6, 3–5.Google Scholar
  8. Cohen, S., & Spacapan, S. (1978). The aftereffects of stress: An attentional interpretation. Environmental Psychology and Nonverbal Behavior, 3, 43–57.Google Scholar
  9. Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1990). Flow: The psychology of optimal experience. New York: Harper & Row.Google Scholar
  10. Csikszentmihalyi, M., & Larson, R. (1987). Validity and reliability of the experience-sampling method. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 175, 526–536.Google Scholar
  11. Davis, R. N., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2000). Cognitive inflexibility among ruminators and nonruminators. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 24, 699–711.Google Scholar
  12. Dittmar, M. L., Warm, J. S., Dember, W. N., & Ricks, D. F. (1993). Sex differences in vigilance performance and perceived workload. Journal of General Psychology, 120, 309–322.Google Scholar
  13. Educational Testing Service. (1995). GRE: Practicing to take the general test: Big book. Princeton, NJ: Warner.Google Scholar
  14. Fennell, M. J., & Teasdale, J. D. (1984). The effects of distraction on thinking and affect in depressed patients. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 23, 65–66.Google Scholar
  15. Gibbons, F. X., Smith, T. W., Ingram, R. E., Pearce, K., Brehm, S. S., & Schroeder, D. (1985). Self-awareness and self-confrontation: Effects of self-focused attention on members of a clinical population. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 48, 662–675.Google Scholar
  16. Gotlib, I. H., Roberts, J. E., & Gilboa, E. (1996). Cognitive interference in depression. In I. G., Sarason, G. R., Pierce, & B. R. Sarason (Eds.), Cognitive interference: Theories, methods, and findings (pp. 347–377). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  17. Granholm, E., Morris, S. K., Sarkin, A. J., Asarnow, R. F., & Jeste, D. V. (1997). Pupillary responses index overload of working memory resources in schizophrenia. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 106, 458–467.Google Scholar
  18. Hartlage, S., Alloy, L. B., Vazquez, C., & Dykman, B. (1993). Automatic and effortful processing in depression. Psychological Bulletin, 113, 247–278.Google Scholar
  19. Hasher, L., & Zacks, R. T. (1979). Automatic and effortful processes in memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 108, 356–388.Google Scholar
  20. Hertel, P. T., & Rude, S. S. (1991). Depressive deficits in memory: Focusing attention improves subsequent recall. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 120, 301–309.Google Scholar
  21. Ingram, R. E. (1990). Self-focused attention in clinical disorders: Review and a conceptual model. Psychological Bulletin, 109, 156–176.Google Scholar
  22. Kuehner, C., & Weber, I. (1999). Responses to depression in unipolar depressed patients: An investigation of Nolen-Hoeksema's response styles theory. Psychological Medicine, 29, 1323–1333.Google Scholar
  23. Kuhl, J., & Helle, P. (1986). Motivational and volitional determinants of depression: The degenerated-intention hypothesis. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 95, 247–251.Google Scholar
  24. Lewinsohn, P. M., Hoberman, H., Teri, L., & Hautzinger, M. (1985). An integrative theory of depression. In S. Reiss & R. Bootzin (Eds.), Theoretical issues in behavior therapy (pp. 331–359). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  25. Lightfoot, S. L., & Oliver, J. M. (1985). The Beck Inventory: Psychometric properties in university students. Journal of Personality Assessment, 49, 434–436.Google Scholar
  26. Linville, P. (1996). Attention inhibition: Does it underlie ruminative thought? In R. S. Wyer Jr. (Ed.), Ruminative thoughts (pp. 121–133). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  27. Lyubomirsky, S., Caldwell, N. D., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (1998). Effects of ruminative and distracting responses to depressed mood on the retrieval of autobiographical memories. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 166–177.Google Scholar
  28. Lyubomirsky, S., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (1993). Self-perpetuating properties of dysphoric rumination. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 339–349.Google Scholar
  29. Lyubomirsky, S., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (1995). Effects of self-focused rumination on negative thinking and interpersonal problem solving. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 176–190.Google Scholar
  30. Lyubomirsky, S., & Tkach, C. (in press). The consequences of dysphoric rumination. In C. Papageorgiou & A. Wells (Eds.), Rumination: Nature, theory, and treatment of negative thinking in depression. Chichester, England: John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
  31. Lyubomirsky, S., Tucker, K. L., Caldwell, N. D., & Berg, K. (1999). Why ruminators are poor problem solvers: Clues from the phenomenology of dysphoric rumination. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77, 1041–1060.Google Scholar
  32. Marascuilo, L. A., McSweeney, M., & Kirk, R. E. (Eds.) (1977). Nonparametric and distribution-free methods for the social sciences. Monterey, CA: Brooks/Cole.Google Scholar
  33. Mikulincer, M. (1989). Cognitive interference and learned helplessness: The effects of off-task cognitions on performance following unsolvable problems. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 129–135.Google Scholar
  34. Morrow, J., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (1990). Effects of responses to depression on the remediation of affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58, 519–527.Google Scholar
  35. Musson, R. F., & Alloy, L. B. (1988). Depression and self-directed attention. In L. B. Alloy (Ed.), Cognitive processes in depression (pp. 163–171). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
  36. Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (1991). Responses to depression and their effects on the duration of depressive episodes. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 100, 569–582.Google Scholar
  37. Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (1996). Chewing the cud and other ruminations. In R. S. Wyer Jr. (Ed.), Ruminative thoughts (pp. 135–144). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  38. Nolen-Hoeksema, S., & Jackson, B. (2000). Personality correlates of rumination. Manuscript submitted for publication.Google Scholar
  39. Nolen-Hoeksema, S., & Morrow, J. (1993). Effects of rumination and distraction on naturally occurring depressed mood. Cognition and Emotion, 7, 561–570.Google Scholar
  40. Ott, R. L. (1993). An introduction to statistical methods and data analysis. Belmont, CA: Duxbury Press.Google Scholar
  41. Papageorgiou, C., & Wells, A. (2001). Metacognitive beliefs about rumination in recurrent major depression. Cognitive and Behavioral Practice, 8, 160–164.Google Scholar
  42. Pierce, G. R., Ptacek, J. T., Taylor, B., Yee, P. L., Henderson, C. A., Lauventi, H. J., et al. (1998). The role of dispositional and situational factors in cognitive interference. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 1016–1031.Google Scholar
  43. Pittman, R. K., Orr, S. P., Forgue, D. F., & Altman, B. (1990). Psychophysiologic responses to combat imagery of Vietnam veterans with posttraumatic stress disorder versus other anxiety disorders. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 99, 49–54.Google Scholar
  44. Pyszczynski, T., Hamilton, J. H., Herring, F., & Greenberg, J. (1989). Depression, self-focused attention, and the negative memory bias. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 351–357.Google Scholar
  45. Rosenthal, R., & Rosnow, R. L. (1985). Contrast analysis. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  46. Rosnow, R. L., & Rosenthal, R. (1995). “Some things you learn aren't so”: Cohen's paradox, Asch's paradigm, and the interpretation of interaction. Psychological Science, 6, 3–9.Google Scholar
  47. Sarason, I. G. (1984). Stress, anxiety, and cognitive interference: Reactions to tests. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 46, 929–938.Google Scholar
  48. Sarason, I. G., Pierce, G. R., & Sarason, B. R. (Eds.) (1996). Cognitive interference: Theories, methods, and findings. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  49. Sarason, I. G., Sarason, B. R., Keefe, D. E., Hayes, B. E., & Shearin, E. N. (1986). Cognitive interference: Situational determinants and traitlike characteristics. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 215–226.Google Scholar
  50. Sarason, I. G., & Stoops, R. (1978). Test anxiety and the passage of time. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 46, 102–109.Google Scholar
  51. Seibert, P. S., & Ellis, H. C. (1991). Irrelevant thoughts, emotional mood states, and cognitive task performance. Memory and Cognition, 19, 507–513.Google Scholar
  52. Selzer, M. L., & Vinokur, A. (1974). Life events, subjective stress and traffic accidents. American Journal of Psychiatry, 131, 903–906.Google Scholar
  53. Smith, T. W., & Greenberg, J. (1981). Depression and self-focused attention. Motivation and Emotion, 5, 323–331.Google Scholar
  54. Strack, F., Blaney, P. H., Ganellen, R. J., & Coyne, J. C. (1985). Pessimistic self-preoccupation, performance deficits, and depression. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 49, 1076–1085.Google Scholar
  55. Teasdale, J. D. (1983). Negative thinking in depression: Cause, effect, or reciprocal relationship? Advances in Behavior Research and Therapy, 5, 3–26.Google Scholar
  56. Ward, A. H., Lyubomirsky, S., Sousa, L., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2003). Can't quite commit: Rumination and uncertainty. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 29, 96–107.Google Scholar
  57. Watkins, E., & Baracaia, S. B. (2001). Why do people ruminate in dysphoric moods? Personality and Individual Differences, 30, 723–734.Google Scholar
  58. Watts, F. N., & Sharrock, R. (1985). Description and measurement of concentration problems in depressed patients. Psychological Medicine, 15, 317–326.Google Scholar
  59. Wenzlaff, R. M., Wegner, D. M., & Klein, S. B. (1991). The role of thought suppression in the bonding of thought and mood. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60, 500–508.Google Scholar
  60. 800score.com. (2002, Spring). Test pacer. In 800score.com: Test prep for the internet age. Retrieved March 29, 2002, from http://www.800score.com/gre-c1p4.html.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Plenum Publishing Corporation 2003

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of PsychologyUniversity of CaliforniaRiverside

Personalised recommendations