Advertisement

Journal of Nonverbal Behavior

, Volume 27, Issue 1, pp 3–23 | Cite as

Emotional Empathy as Related to Mimicry Reactions at Different Levels of Information Processing

  • Marianne Sonnby-Borgström
  • Peter Jönsson
  • Owe Svensson
Article

Abstract

The hypotheses of this investigation were based on conceiving of facial mimicry reactions in face-to-face interactions as an early automatic component in the process of emotional empathy. Differences between individuals high and low in emotional empathy were investigated. The parameters compared were facial mimicry reactions, as represented by electromyographic (EMG) activity, when individuals were exposed to pictures of angry or happy faces. The present study distinguished between spontaneous facial reactions and facial expressions associated with more controlled or modulated emotions at different information processing levels, first at a preattentive level and then consecutively at more consciously controlled levels: 61 participants were exposed to pictures at three different exposure times (17, 56, and 2350 ms). A significant difference in facial mimicry reactions between high- and low-empathy participants emerged at short exposure times (56 ms), representing automatic, spontaneous reactions, with high-empathy participants showing a significant mimicking reaction. The low-empathy participants did not display mimicking at any exposure time. On the contrary, the low-empathy participants showed, in response to angry faces, a tendency to an elevated activation in the cheek region, which often is associated with smiling.

EMG emotional contagion empathy facial expressions facial mimicry mirror neurons 

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Bäckström, M., & Holmes, B. (1999). Measuring adult attachment: A construct validation of two self-report instruments. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 42, 79–86.Google Scholar
  2. Barnett, M. A., Howard, J. A., King, L. M., & Dino, G. A. (1981). Helping behaviour and the transfer of empathy. The Journal of Social Psychology, 115, 125–132.Google Scholar
  3. Bartholomew, K., & Horowitz, L. M. (1991). Attachment styles among young adults: A test of a four-category model. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61, 226–244.Google Scholar
  4. Basch, M. F. (1976). Psychoanalysis and communication science. The Annals of Psychoanalysis, 31, 101–126.Google Scholar
  5. Basch, M. F. (1983). Empathic understanding: A review of the concept and some theoretical considerations. Journal of the American the Psychoanalytical Association, 31, 101–126.Google Scholar
  6. Bavelas, J. B., Black, A., Lemery, C. R., & Mullett, J. (1986). "I show how you feel": Motor mimicry as a communicative act. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50, 322–329.Google Scholar
  7. Blairy, S., Herrera, P., & Hess, U. (1999). Mimicry and the judgement of emotional facial expressions. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 23, 5–41.Google Scholar
  8. Brown, J. W. (1985). Clinical evidence for the concept of levels of action and perception. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 1, 89–141.Google Scholar
  9. Brown, J. W. (1988). Life of the mind. Selected papers. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.Google Scholar
  10. Brown, S. L., & Schwartz, G. E. (1980). Relationships between facial electromyography and subjective experience during affective imagery. Biological Psychology, 11, 49–62.Google Scholar
  11. Buck, R. (1984). The communication of emotion. New York: Guilford.Google Scholar
  12. Buck, R. (1994). The neuropsychology of communication: Spontaneous and symbolic aspects. Journal of Pragmatics, 22, 256–278.Google Scholar
  13. Buck, R., & Ginsberg, B. (1991). Emotional communication and altruism: The communicative gene hypotheses. In M. S. Clark (Ed.), Altruism. Review of personality and social psychology (Vol. 11, pp. 149–175). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
  14. Burgoon, J. K., Buller, D. B., & Woodall, W. G. (1996). Nonverbal communication: The unspoken dialogue. New York: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
  15. Cacioppo, J. T., Klein, D. J., Berntsson, G. G., & Hatfield, E. (1993). The psychophysiology of emotion. In M. Lewis & J. M. Haviland (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (pp. 119–142). New York: The Guilford Press.Google Scholar
  16. Cacioppo, J. T., Matzke, J. S., Petty, R. E., & Tassinary, L. G. (1988). Specific forms of facial EMG response index emotions during an interview: From Darwin to the continuous flow of affect laden information processing. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 592–604.Google Scholar
  17. Cacioppo, J. T., Petty, R. E., Losch, M. E., & Kim, H. S. (1986). Electromyographic activity over facial muscle regions can differentiate the valence and the intensity of affective reactions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50, 260–268.Google Scholar
  18. Capella, J. N. (1993). The facial feed back hypotheses in human interaction: Review and speculation. Journal of Language and Social Psychology, 12, 13–29.Google Scholar
  19. Chartrand, T. L., & Bargh, J. A. (1999). The chameleon effect: The perception-behavior link and social interaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76, 893–910.Google Scholar
  20. Chlopan, B. E., McCain, M. L., Carbonell, J. L., & Hagen, R. L. (1985). Empathy: Review of available measures. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 48, 635–653.Google Scholar
  21. Darwin, C. (1872, 1965). The expression of emotion in man and in animals. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
  22. Dimberg, U. (1982). Facial reactions to facial expressions. Psychophysiology, 19, 643–647.Google Scholar
  23. Dimberg, U. (1989). Facial expressions and emotional reactions: A psychobiological analysis of human social behaviour. In H. L. Wagner (Ed.), Social psychophysiology and emotion: Theory and clinical applications (Vol. 36, pp. 132–149). London: John Wiley & Sons Ltd.Google Scholar
  24. Dimberg, U. (1997a). Psychophysiological reactions to facial expressions. In U. Segerstråle & P. Molnar (Eds.), Nonverbal communication: Where nature meets culture (pp. 47–60). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.Google Scholar
  25. Dimberg, U. (1997b). Social fear and expressive reactions to social stimuli. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 38, 171–174.Google Scholar
  26. Dimberg, U., & Karlsson, B. (1997). Facial reactions to different emotionally relevant stimuli. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 38, 297–303.Google Scholar
  27. Dimberg, U., & Öhman, A. (1996). Behold the wrath: Psychophysiological responses to facial stimuli. Motivation and Emotion, 20, 149–182.Google Scholar
  28. Dimberg, U., Thunberg, M., & Elmehed, K. (2000). Unconscious facial reactions to emotional facial expressions. Psychological Science, 11, 86–89.Google Scholar
  29. Dixon, N. F. (1981). Preconscious processing. New York: John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
  30. Eisenberg, N., & Fabes, R. (1990). Empathy: Conceptualization, measurement, and relation to prosocial behavior. Motivation and Emotion, 14, 131–150.Google Scholar
  31. Eisenberg-Berg, N., & Mussen, P. (1978). Empathy and moral development in adolescence. Developmental Psychology, 14, 185–186.Google Scholar
  32. Ekman, P. (1989). The argument and evidence about universals in facial expressions of emotion. In J. Wagner & A. Manstead (Eds.), Handbook of social pychophysiology (pp. 143–164). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  33. Ekman, P. (1994). Strong evidence for universals in facial expressions: A reply to Russell's mistaken critique. Psychological Bulletin, 115, 268–287.Google Scholar
  34. Ekman, P., & Friesen, W. V. (1969). The repertoire of nonverbal behavior: Categories, usage, and coding. Semiotica, 1, 49–98.Google Scholar
  35. Ekman, P., & Friesen, W. V. (1975). Unmasking the face. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.Google Scholar
  36. Ekman, P., & Friesen, W. (1978). Facial action coding system: A technique for the measurement of facial movement. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychology Press.Google Scholar
  37. Ekman, P., & Friesen, W. V. (1982). Felt, false, and miserable smiles. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 6, 638–252.Google Scholar
  38. Ekman, P., Friesen, W. V., & Ancoli, S. (1980). Facial signs of emotional experience. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39, 1125–1134.Google Scholar
  39. Ekman, P., Levenson, R., & Friesen, W. V. (1983). Autonomic nervous system activity distinguishes among emotions. Science, 221, 1208–1210.Google Scholar
  40. Englis, B. G., Vaughan, K. B., & Lanzetta, J. T. (1982). Conditioning of counterempathic emotional responses. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 18, 375–391.Google Scholar
  41. Esteves, F., & Öhman, A. (1993). Masking the face: Recognition of emotional facial expressions as a function of the parameters of backward masking. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 34, 1–18.Google Scholar
  42. Eysenck, M. W., & Keane, M. T. (1995). Cognitive psychology. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers.Google Scholar
  43. Eysenck, S. B. G., & Eysenck, H. J. (1978). Impulsiveness and venturesomeness: Their position in a dimensional system of personality description. Psychological Reports, 43, 1247–1255.Google Scholar
  44. Fridlund, A. J., & Cacioppo, J. T. (1986). Guidelines for human electromyographic research. Psychophysiology, 23, 567–589.Google Scholar
  45. Gallese, V. (2001). The "shared manifold" hypothesis: From mirror neurons to empathy. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 8, 33–50.Google Scholar
  46. Ginsburg, G. P. (1997). Faces: An epilogue and reconceptualization. In J. A. Russell & J. M. Fernández-Dols (Eds.), The psychology of facial expression (pp. 349–382). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  47. Griffin, D., & Bartholomew, K. (1994). The metaphysics of measurement: The case of adult attachment. In K. Bartholomew, & D. Perlman (Eds.), Attachment processes in adulthood: Advances in personal relationships (Vol. 5, pp. 17–52). London, UK: Jessica Kingsley Publisher Ltd.Google Scholar
  48. Hatfield, E., Cacioppo, J., & Rapson, R. L. (1992). Emotional contagion. In M. S. Clark (Ed.), Review of personality and social psychology: Emotion and social behavior (Vol. 14, pp. 151–177). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
  49. Hatfield, E., Cacioppo, J. T., & Rapson, R. L. (1994). Emotional contagion. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  50. Hess, U., Banse, R., & Kappas, A. (1995). The intensity of facial expression is determined by underlying affect and the social situation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 280–288.Google Scholar
  51. Hess, U., Kappas, A., McHugo, G. J., Lanzetta, J. T., & Kleck, R. E. (1992). The facilitative effects of facial expressions on the self-generation of emotion. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 12, 251–265.Google Scholar
  52. Hess, U., Philippot, P., & Blairy, S. (1998). Facial reactions to emotional facial expressions: Affect or cognition? Cognition and Emotion, 12, 509–531.Google Scholar
  53. Hess, U., Philippot, P., & Blairy, S. (1999). Mimicry: Facts and fictions. In R. S. Philippot, R. S. Feldman, & E. J. Coats (Eds.), The social context of nonverbal behavior (pp. 213–241). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  54. Hjortsjö, C. H. (1970). Man's face and mimic language. Malmö: Nordens Boktryckeri.Google Scholar
  55. Hofer, M. (1984). Relationships as regulators: A psychobiologic perspective on bereavement. Psychosomatic Medicine, 46, 183–197.Google Scholar
  56. Hoffman, M. L. (1984). Interaction of affect and cognition on empathy. In C. E. Izard, J. Kagan, & R. B. Zajonc (Eds.), Emotions, Cognition and Behavior (pp. 101–131). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  57. Hsee, C. K., Hatfield, E., & Chemtob, C. (1992). Assessments of the emotional states of others: Conscious judgements versus emotional contagion. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 11, 119–128.Google Scholar
  58. Izard, C. E. (1971). The face of emotion. New York: Appelton-Century-Crofts.Google Scholar
  59. Izard, C. E. (1990). Facial expressions and the regulation of emotions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58, 487–498.Google Scholar
  60. Izard, C. E. (1994). Innate and universal facial expressions: Evidence from developmental and cross-cultural research. Psychological Bulletin, 115, 288–299.Google Scholar
  61. Kappas, A., Hess, U., & Banse, R. (1992). Skin conductance reactions to dynamic facial expressions revisited: Empathic responding or information processing? Psychophysiology, 29, 42.Google Scholar
  62. Keltner, D., & Ekman, P. (2000). Facial expression of emotion. In M. Lewis & J. M. Haviland-Jones (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (pp. 236–249). New York: The Guilford Press.Google Scholar
  63. Kragh, U., & Smith, G. J. W. (1970). Percept-genetic analysis. Lund, Sweden: Gleerup.Google Scholar
  64. Laird, J. D. (1974). Self-attribution of emotion: The effects of expressive behavior on the quality of emotional experience. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 475–486.Google Scholar
  65. Laird, J. D., Alibozak, T., Davainis, D., Deignan, K., Fontanella, K., Hong, J., Brett, L., & Pacheco, C. (1994). Individual differences in the effects of spontaneous mimicry on emotional contagion. Motivation and Emotion, 18, 231–245.Google Scholar
  66. Lang, P. J., Greenwald, M. K., Bradley, M. M., & Hamm, A. O. (1993). Looking at pictures: Affective, facial, visceral and behavioral reactions. Psychophysiology, 30, 261–273.Google Scholar
  67. Lanzetta, J. T., & Englis, B. G. (1989). Expectation of cooperation and competition and their effects on observers' vicarious emotional responses. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 46, 543–554.Google Scholar
  68. Lanzetta, J. T., & Kleck, R. E. (1976). Effects of nonverbal dissimulation on emotional experience and autonomic arousal. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 33, 354–370.Google Scholar
  69. LeDoux, J. (1996). The emotional brain, The mysterious underpinnings of emotional life. New York: Simon & Schuster.Google Scholar
  70. Levenson, R. W. (1996). Biological substrates of empathy and facial modulation of emotion: Two facets of the scientific legacy of John Lanzetta. Motivation and Emotion, 20, 185–204.Google Scholar
  71. Leventhal, H. (1984). A perceptual motor theory of emotion. Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 17, pp. 117–182). Madison, WI: Academic Press, Inc.Google Scholar
  72. Lipps, T. (1903). Kapitel: Die einfühlung in leitfaden der psychology. Leipzig: Verlag von Wilhem Engelmann.Google Scholar
  73. Lundqvist, L.-O. (1995). Facial EMG reactions to facial expressions: A case of emotional contagion? Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 36, 130–141.Google Scholar
  74. Matsumoto, D. (1987). The role of the facial response in experience of emotion: More methodological problems and meta-analysis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 769–774.Google Scholar
  75. Matsumoto, D., & Lee, M. (1991). Consciousness, volition and the neuropsychology of facial expression and emotion. Consciousness and Cognition, 2, 237–254.Google Scholar
  76. McHugo, G. J., & Smith, C. A. (1996). The power of faces: A review of John T. Lanzetta's research on facial expression and emotion. Motivation and Emotion, 20, 85–119.Google Scholar
  77. McIntosh, D. N. (1996). Facial feedback hypotheses: Evidence, implications, and directions. Motivation and Emotion, 20, 121–147.Google Scholar
  78. Mehrabian, A., & Epstein, N. (1972). A measure of emotional empathy. Journal of Personality, 40, 525–543.Google Scholar
  79. Öhman, A. (1993). Fear and anxiety as emotional phenomena: Clinical phenomenology, evolutionary perspectives and information processing mechanisms. In M. Lewis & J. M. Haviland (Eds.), Handbook of emotion (pp. 511–536). New York: The Guilford Press.Google Scholar
  80. Öhman, A., & Dimberg, U. (1978). Facial expression as conditioned stimuli for electrodermal responses. A case of "preparedness"? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 36, 1251–1258.Google Scholar
  81. Pally, R. (1998). Emotional processing: The mind-body connection. Journal of Psychoanalysis, 79, 349–362.Google Scholar
  82. Porges, S. W. (1991). Vagal tone: An automatic mediator of affect. In J. Garber & K. A. Dodge (Eds.), The development of emotion regulation and dysregulation (pp. 111–127). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  83. Schore, A. N. (1994). Affect regulation and the origin of the self. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publisher.Google Scholar
  84. Sonnby-Borgström, M. (2002a). Between Ourselves. Automatic mimicry reactions as related to empathic ability and patterns of attachment. Doctoral thesis, Lund University, Lund.Google Scholar
  85. Sonnby-Borgström, M. (2002b). Automatic mimicry reactions as related to differences in emotional empathy. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 43, 433–443.Google Scholar
  86. Sonnby-Borgström, M., & Jönsson, P. (2003). Model-of-self and model-of-others as related to facial muscle reactions at different levels of cognitive control. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 44, 153–163Google Scholar
  87. Tassinary, L. G., & Cacioppo, J. T. (1992). Unobservable facial actions and emotion. Psychological Science, 3, 28–33.Google Scholar
  88. Tassinary, L. G., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2000). The skeletomotor system: Surface electromyography. In J. T. Cacioppo, L. G. Tassinary, & G. G. Berntsson (Eds.), Handbook of psychophysiology (pp. 163–199). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  89. Tassinary, L. G., Cacioppo, J. T., & Geen, T. R. (1989). A psychometric study of surface electrode placements for facial electromyographic recording. Psychophysiology, 24, 1–16.Google Scholar
  90. Tassinary, L. G., Scott, P. O., Wolford, G., Napps, S. E., & Lanzetta, J. T. (1984). The role of awareness in affective information processing: An exploration of the Zajonc hypothesis. Bullentin of Psychonomic Society, 22, 489–492.Google Scholar
  91. Tomkins, S. (1984). Affect theory. In K. Scherer & P. Ekman (Eds.), Approaches to emotion (pp. 353–400). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  92. Tomkins, S. (1991). Affect, imagery and consciousness. New York: Springer Publishing Company.Google Scholar
  93. Trevarthen, C. (2001). Intrinsic motives for companionship in understanding: Their origin, development, and significance for infant mental health. Infant Mental Health, 22(1–2), 95–131.Google Scholar
  94. Vaughan, K. B., & Lanzetta, J. T. (1980). Vicarious instigation and conditioning of facial expressive automatic responses to a model's expressive display of pain. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 38, 909–923.Google Scholar
  95. Williams, J. H. G., Whiten, A., Suddendorf, T., & Perrett, D. I. (2001). Imitation, mirror neurons and autism. Neuroscience and Biobehavioural Reviews, 25, 287–295.Google Scholar
  96. Wolf, N. S., Gales, M. E., Shane, E., & Shane, M. (2001). The developmental trajectory from amodal perception to empathy and communication: The role of mirror neurons in this process. Psychoanalytic Inquiry, 21, 94–112.Google Scholar
  97. Wood, J. V., Salzberg, J. A., & Goldsamt, L. A. (1990). Does affect induce self-focused attention? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58, 899–908.Google Scholar
  98. Vrana, R. S., & Rollock, D. (1998). Physiological response to a minimal social encounter: Effects of gender, ethnicity and social context. Phychophysiology, 35, 462–469.Google Scholar
  99. Zajonc, R. B. (1980). Feeling and thinking. Preferences need no inferences. American Psychologist, 35, 151–175.Google Scholar
  100. Zajonc, R. B. (1985). Emotion and facial efference: A theory reclaimed. Science, 228, 15–21.Google Scholar
  101. Zajonc, R. B., Adelmann, K. A., Murphy, S. T., & Niedenthal, P. M. (1987). Convergence of the physical appearence of spouses. Motivation and Emotion, 11, 335–346.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Human Sciences Press, Inc. 2003

Authors and Affiliations

  • Marianne Sonnby-Borgström
    • 1
  • Peter Jönsson
    • 1
  • Owe Svensson
    • 1
  1. 1.Department of PsychologyLund UniversitySweden

Personalised recommendations