Advertisement

Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology

, Volume 27, Issue 1, pp 25–33 | Cite as

Heart Rate Variability and Sustained Attention in ADHD Children

  • Norbert Börger
  • Jaap van Der Meere
  • Arjen Ronner
  • Ed Alberts
  • Reint Geuze
  • Hans Bogte
Article

Abstract

The major goal of the current study was to investigate the association between continuous performance tests (CPTs) and the heart rate variability (HRV) of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) children. The HRV, specifically the 0.10-Hz component, may be considered to be a psychophysiological index of effort allocation (motivation): The less effort the subject allocates, the greater the 0.10-Hz component. Results indicated that, compared to controls, ADHD subjects had a greater 0.10-Hz component, which was associated with poor test performance over time. Thus, using a psychophysiological measure, we were able to confirm the clinical concept of ADHD from a motivational perspective.

ADHD sustained attention heart rate variability 

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

REFERENCES

  1. Alberts, E., & Van der Meere, J. J. (1992). Observations of hyperactive behavior during vigilance. The Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 33, 1355–1364.Google Scholar
  2. American Psychiatric Association. (1987). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (3rd ed., rev.). Washington, DC: Author.Google Scholar
  3. American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: Author.Google Scholar
  4. Barkley, R. A. (1997). Behavioral inhibition, sustained attention, and executive functions: Constructing a unifying theory of ADHD. Psychological Bulletin, 121, 65–94.Google Scholar
  5. Beh, H. C. (1990). Achievement motivation, performance and cardiovascular activity. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 10, 39–45.Google Scholar
  6. Börger, N. A., Van der Meere, J. J., & Wiersema, R. (in press). Heartrate variability and sustained attention (Abstract). Journal of Psychophysiology.Google Scholar
  7. Brouwer, W. H., & Van Wolffelaar, P. C. (1985). Sustained attention and sustained effort after closed head injury: Detection and 0.10 Hz heart rate variability in a low event rate vigilance task. Cortex, XXI, 111–119.Google Scholar
  8. Carter, N. L., & Beh, H. C. (1989). The effect of intermittent noise on cardiovascular functioning during vigilance task performance. Psychophysiology, 26, 548–558.Google Scholar
  9. Cohen, J. (1968). Weighted kappa: Nominal scale agreement with provision for scaled disagreement or partial credit. Psychological Bulletin, 70, 213–320.Google Scholar
  10. Egelund, N. (1982). Spectral analysis of heart rate variability as an indicator of driver fatigue. Ergonomics, 25, 663–672.Google Scholar
  11. Fairclough, S. H. (1993). Psychophysiological measures of workload and stress. In A. M. Parkes & S. Franzèn (Eds.), Driving future vehicles (pp. 207–216). London, England: Taylor & Francis.Google Scholar
  12. Gross-Tsur, V., Manor, O., Van der Meere, J. J., Joseph, A, & Shalev, R. (1997). Epilepsy and ADHD: Is methylphenidate safe and effective? Journal of Pediatrics, 130, 40–44.Google Scholar
  13. Jorna, P. G. A. M. (1985). Heart rate parameters and the coping process underwater. In J. F. Orlebeke, G. Mulder, & L. J. P. Van Doornen (Eds.), Psychophysiology of cardiovascular control: methods, Models, and data (pp. 827–839). New York: Plenum Press.Google Scholar
  14. Jorna, P. G. A. M. (1992). Spectral analysis of heart rate and psychological state: A review of its validity as a workload index. Biological Psychology, 34, 237–257.Google Scholar
  15. Jorna, P. G. A. M., & Mulder, G. (1983). Mental load and the processing of information in the underwater environment. In H. Ursin (Ed.), Biological and psychological bases of psychosomatic disease. Oxford, England: Pergamon.Google Scholar
  16. Kamphuis, A., & Frowein, H. W. (1985). Assessment of mental effort by means of heart rate spectral analysis. In J. F. Orlebeke, G. Mulder, & L. P. J. van Doornen (Eds.), The psychophysiology of cardiovascular control (pp. 841–853). New York: Plenum Press.Google Scholar
  17. Keidel, W. D. (1979). Kurzgefasstes lehrbuch der Physiologie. Stuttgart, Germany: Georg Thieme Verlag.Google Scholar
  18. Kupietz, S. S. (1990). Sustained attention in normal and in reading disabled youngsters with and without ADDH. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 18, 357–372.Google Scholar
  19. Mulder, G. (1986). The concept and measurement of mental effort. In G. R. J. Hockey, A. W. K. Gaillard, & M. G. H. Coles (Eds.), Energetics and human information processing (pp. 175–198). Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Martinus Nijhoff.Google Scholar
  20. Mulder, L. J. M., Van Dellen, H. J., Van der Meulen, P., & Opheikens, B. (1988). CARSPAN: A spectral analysis program for cardiovascular time series. In F. J. Maarse, L. J. M. Mulder, W. Sjouw, & A. Akkerman (Eds.), Computers in psychology: Methods, instrumentation & psychodiagnostics (pp. 30–38). Lisse, The Nethelands: Swets & Zeitlinger.Google Scholar
  21. Mulder, L. J. M., Veldman, J. P. B., van der Veen, F. M., Van Roon, A. M., Rüddel, H., Schächinger, H., & Mulder, G. (1992). On the effects of mental task performance on heart rate, blood pressure and its variability measures. In M. di Rienzo, G. Mancia, G. Parati, A. Pedotti, & A. Zanchetti (Eds.), Blood pressure and heart rate variability (pp. 153–166). Amsterdam, The Netherlands: IOS Press.Google Scholar
  22. Parasuraman, R. (1979). Memory load and event rate control sensitivity decrements in sustained attention. Science, 205, 924–927.Google Scholar
  23. Parasuraman, R. (1984). The psychobiology of sustained attention. In J. S. Warm (Ed.), Sustained attention in human performance (pp. 61–101). London, England: John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
  24. Parasuraman, R., & Davies, D. R. (1979). A taxonomic analysis of vigilance performance. In R. R. Mackie (Ed.), Vigilance: Theory, operational performance and physiological correlates (pp. 559–574). New York: Plenum Press.Google Scholar
  25. Porges, S. W., & Byrne, E. A. (1992). Research methods for measurement of heart rate and respiration. Biological Psychology, 34, 93–130.Google Scholar
  26. Posner, M. I., & Raichle, M. E. (1994). Images of mind. New York: Scientific American Library.Google Scholar
  27. Rompelman, O., Coenen, A. J. R. M., & Kitney, R. I. (1977). Measurement of heart rate variability. Part I—Comparative study of heart rate variability analysis methods. Medical and Biological Engineering and Computing, 15, 233.Google Scholar
  28. Skinner, J. E. (1991). Brain control of cardiovascular dynamics. In C. H. M. Brunia, G. Mulder, & M. N. Verbaten (Eds.), Event related brain research (EEG Suppl. 42) (pp. 270–283). Amsterdam, The Netherlands: Elsevier.Google Scholar
  29. Suess, P. E., Porges, S. W., & Plude, D. J. (1994). Cardiac vagal tone and sustained attention in school-age children. Psychophysiology, 31, 17–22.Google Scholar
  30. Thackray, R. I., Bailey, J. P., & Touchstone, R. M. (1979). The effect of increased monitoring load on vigilance performance using a simulated radar display. Ergonomics, 22, 529–539.Google Scholar
  31. Vaessen, W., & Van der Meere, J. J. (1990). Issues in the selection of hyperactive/ADD(H) children for experimental clinical studies. In A. F. Kalverboer (Ed.), Developmental biopsychology: Experimental and observational studies in groups “at risk” (pp. 21–37). Ann Arbor: The University of Michigan Press.Google Scholar
  32. Van der Mark, R., & Geuze, R. H. (1988). Eventlib: A software package for the acquisition and the analysis of observations. In F. J. Maarse, L. J. M. Mulder, W. P. B. Sjouw, & A. E. Akkerman (Eds.), Computers in psychology: Methods, instruments and psychodiagnostics (pp. 1–15). Lisse, The Netherlands: Swets & Zeitlinger.Google Scholar
  33. Van der Meere, J. J. (1996). The role of inattention in hyperactivty disorders. In S. Sandberg (Ed.), Monographs on child and adolescent psychiatry: Hyperactivity disorders (pp. 109–146). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  34. Van der Meere, J. J., Hughes, K. A., Börger, N. A., & Sallee, F. L. (1995). The effect of reward on sustained attention in ADHD children with and without CD. In J. A. Sergeant (Ed.), Eunethydis, European approaches to hyperkinetic disorder (pp. 241–253). Zurich, Switserland: Trümpi & Partner.Google Scholar
  35. Van der Meere, J. J., & Sergeant, J. A. (1987). A divided attention experiment with pervasively hyperactive children. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 15, 379–391.Google Scholar
  36. Van der Meere, J. J., & Sergeant, J. A. (1988). Controlled processing and vigilance in hyperactivity: Time will tell. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 16, 641–655.Google Scholar
  37. Van der Meere, J. J., Shalev, R., Börger, N. A., & Gross-Tsur, V. (1995). Sustained attention, activation and MPH in ADHD: A research note. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 36, 697–703.Google Scholar
  38. Wesseling, K. H., & Settels, J. J. (1985). Baromodulation explains short-term blood pressure variability. In J. F. Orlebeke, G. Mulder & L. P. J. van Doornen (Eds.), The psychophysiology of cardiovascular control (pp. 69–97). New York: Plenum Press.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Plenum Publishing Corporation 1999

Authors and Affiliations

  • Norbert Börger
    • 1
  • Jaap van Der Meere
    • 1
  • Arjen Ronner
    • 1
  • Ed Alberts
    • 1
  • Reint Geuze
    • 1
  • Hans Bogte
    • 2
  1. 1.Laboratory of Experimental Clinical PsychologyGroningenThe Netherlands
  2. 2.Universitair Centrum Groningen, Kinder en JeugdpsychiatrieGroningenThe Netherlands

Personalised recommendations