Motivation and Emotion

, Volume 22, Issue 3, pp 255–275 | Cite as

A Time-Series Diary Study of Mood and Social Interaction

  • Jeffrey R. Vittengl
  • Craig S. Holt

Abstract

Based on concomitant time-series analyses, the results of this study support distinct social interaction correlates for the mood dimensions of negative affect (NA) and positive affect (PA). Participants (N = 25) completed structured diaries three times daily for 4 weeks assessing their PA, NA, and participation in five types of social interaction. A significant number of participants' data series evidenced significant positive correlations between PA and fun/active and necessary/informational types of social interaction, and between NA and arguing/confronting and receiving help/support, during synchronous diary periods. Providing help/support was not related to NA or PA. No hypothesized time-lagged relations between mood and social interaction variables were present suggesting that, if these associations exist, they may be at intervals shorter than the one third day recording frequency used in this study. Results are discussed in the context of research on mood, social interaction, and time-series analysis.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

REFERENCES

  1. Barrera, M., Jr. (1986). Distinctions between social support concepts, measures, and models. American Journal of Community Psychology, 14 413–445.Google Scholar
  2. Bower, M. E., & Holt, C. S. (1996, November). Social support and the social environment as a function of neuroticism and social anxiety. Poster presented at the annual meeting of the Association for Advancement of Behavior Therapy.Google Scholar
  3. Clark, L. A., & Watson, D. (1988). Mood and the mundane: Relations between daily life events and self-reported mood. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54 296–308.Google Scholar
  4. Clark, L. A., Watson, D., & Leeka, J. (1989). Diurnal variation in the positive affects. Motivation and Emotion, 13 205–234.Google Scholar
  5. Cohen, J. (1977). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (Rev. ed.). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  6. Cohen, S., & Wills, T. A. (1985). Stress, social support, and the buffering hypothesis. Psychological Bulletin, 98 310–357.Google Scholar
  7. Cromwell, J. B., Labys, W. C., & Terraza, M. (1994). Univariate tests for time series models. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
  8. Cunningham, M. R. (1988a). Does happiness mean friendliness?: Induced mood and heterosexual self-disclosure. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 14 283–297.Google Scholar
  9. Cunningham, M. R. (1988b). What do you do when you're happy or blue? Mood, expectancies, and behavioral interest. Motivation and Emotion, 12 309–331.Google Scholar
  10. Cutrona, C. E. (1986). Behavioral manifestations of social support: A microanalytic investigation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51 201–208.Google Scholar
  11. Duck, S., Rutt, D. J., Hurst, M. H., & Strejc, H. (1991). Some evident truths about conversations in everyday relationships: All communications are not created equal. Human Communication Research, 18 228–267.Google Scholar
  12. Gottman, J. M. (1981). Time-series analysis: A comprehensive introduction for social scientists. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  13. Hokanson, J. E., Tate, R. L., Niu, X., Stader, S. R., & Flynn, H. A. (1994). Illustration of concomitant time series analyses in a case of somatoform pain disorder. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 18 413–437.Google Scholar
  14. Kennedy-Moore, E., Greenberg, M. A., Newman, M. G., & Stone, A. A. (1992). The relationship between daily events and mood: The mood measure may matter. Motivation and Emotion, 16 143–155.Google Scholar
  15. McCleary, R., & Hay, R. A., Jr. (1980). Applied time series analysis for the social sciences. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
  16. McIntyre, C. W., Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Cross, S. A. (1991). The effect of induced social interaction on positive and negative affect. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 29 67–70.Google Scholar
  17. McIntyre, C. W., Watson, D., & Cunningham, A. C. (1990). The effects of social interaction, exercise, and test stress on positive and negative affect. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 28 141–143.Google Scholar
  18. Rushe, R. H., & Gottman, J. M. (1993). Essentials in the design and analysis of time-series experiments. In G. Keren & C. Lewis (Eds.), A handbook for data analysis in the behavioral sciences: Statistical issues (pp. 493–528). Hillsdale, NJ: Earlbaum.Google Scholar
  19. SPSS Inc. (1994). SPSS 6.1 (Computer software). Chicago: Author.Google Scholar
  20. Stone, A. A., Hedges, S. M., Neale, J. M., & Satin, M. S. (1985). Prospective and cross-sectional mood reports offer no evidence of a “blue Monday” phenomenon. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 49 129–134.Google Scholar
  21. Stone, A. A., Smyth, J. M., Pickering, T., & Schwartz, J. (1996). Daily mood variability: Form of diurnal patterns and determinants of diurnal patterns. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 26 1286–1305.Google Scholar
  22. Thoits, P. A. (1986). Social support as coping assistance. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 54 416–423.Google Scholar
  23. Vinokur, A., Schul, Y., & Caplan, R. D. (1987). Determinants of perceived social support: Interpersonal transactions, personal outlook, and transient affective states. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53 1137–1145.Google Scholar
  24. Vittengl, J. R., & Holt, C. S. (1998). Positive and negative affect in social interactions as a function of partner familiarity, quality of communication, and social anxiety. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 17 196–208.Google Scholar
  25. Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Carey, G. (1988). Positive and negative affectivity and their relation to anxiety and depressive disorders. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 97 346–353.Google Scholar
  26. Watson, D., Clark, L. A., McIntyre, C. W., & Hamaker, S. (1992). Affect, personality, and social activity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63 1011–1025.Google Scholar
  27. Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief measures of Positive and Negative Affect: The PANAS Scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54 1063–1070.Google Scholar
  28. Watson, D., & Tellegen, A. (1985). Toward a consensual structure of mood. Psychological Bulletin, 98 219–235.Google Scholar
  29. West, S. G., & Hepworth, J. T. (1991). Statistical issues in the study of temporal data: Daily experiences. Journal of Personality, 59 609–662.Google Scholar
  30. Wheeler, L., & Reis, H. T. (1991). Self-recording of everyday life events: Origins, types, and uses. Journal of Personality, 59 339–354.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Plenum Publishing Corporation 1998

Authors and Affiliations

  • Jeffrey R. Vittengl
    • 3
  • Craig S. Holt
    • 1
    • 2
  1. 1.University of IowaIowa City
  2. 2.Iowa City Veterans Affairs Medical CenterIowa City
  3. 3.Department of PsychologyUniversity of IowaIowa City

Personalised recommendations