Advertisement

Genetica

, 99:125 | Cite as

Hereditarian scientific fallacies

  • Robert C. Bailey
Article
  • 49 Downloads

Abstract

Some have recently declared that a hereditarian or more balanced approach has triumphed over environmentalism as an explanatory tool for variation in the cognitive ability and behaviour of humans. However, the entire debate is constrained by several fallacies described here. Heritability of a trait does not predict the effect of environmental or genetic changes on the trait (Fallacy #1), so knowing heritability does not assist in writing prescriptions for societal ills or budget cuts. Heritability estimates themselves are inaccurate, given the potential for gene-environment covariance and interaction, as well as other non-additive effects on behavior or cognitive ability (Fallacy #2). The ‘revolution in molecular genetics’ has provided more effective tools for describing the genome, but doesn't permit separation of gene and environmental effects on traits (Fallacy #3). If we were able to measure heritability accurately, it would give us absolutely no indication of whether or not group differences are genetically based (Fallacy #4). Finally, any proposed models of the evolutionary divergence of human groups must more adequately answer the basic questions of such a study, and are not supported by high heritability in present populations (Fallacy #5). Humans are not and should never be exposed to artificial selection and crossing experiments, so behavior geneticists will continue to be very limited in their ability to partition the effects of genes, the environment, and their covariance and interaction on human behavior and cognitive ability.

behavioral genetics environmentalist hereditarian nature vs nurture 

References

  1. Ankney, C.D., July 6, 1994. What if violent types are born, not made? Facts and Arguments. Toronto Globe and Mail.Google Scholar
  2. Benjamin, J., L. Li, C. Patterson, B.D. Greenberg, D.L. Murphy & D.H. Hamer, 1996. Population and familial association between the D4 dopamine receptor gene and measures of novelty seeking. Nature Genetics 12: 81–84.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Bouchard, T.J., Jr., D.T. Lykken, M. McGue, N.L. Segal & A. Tellegen, 1990. Sources of human psychological differences: the Minnesota study of twins reared apart. Science 250: 223–228.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  4. Ebstein, R.P., O. Novick, R. Umansky, B. Priel, Y. Osher, D. Blaine, E.R. Bennett, L. Nemanov, M. Katz, R.H. Belmaker, 1996. Dopamine D4 receptor (D4DR) exon III polymorphism associated with the human personality trait of novelty seeking. Nature Genetics 12: 78–80.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Gallagher, W., September 1994. How we become what we are. The Atlantic Monthly 273: 39–54.Google Scholar
  6. Gould, S.J. & R.C. Lewontin, 1979. The spandrels of San Marco and the panglossian paradigm: a critique of the adaptationist program. Proc. R. Soc. London B. 205: 581–598.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Herrnstein, R.J. & C. Murray, 1994. The Bell Curve. Free Press, New York.Google Scholar
  8. Hirsch, J., 1981. To 'unfrock the charlatans'. SAGE Race Relations Abstract 6: 1–65.Google Scholar
  9. Kevles, D.J., 1985. In the Name of Eugenics. Alfred A. Knopf, New York.Google Scholar
  10. Lande, R., 1976. The maintenance of genetic variability by mutation in a polygenic character with linked loci. Genet. Res. 26: 221–235.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Mather, K., 1943. Polygenic inheritance and natural selection. Biol. Rev. 18: 32–64.Google Scholar
  12. Plomin, R., M.J. Owen & P. McGuffin, 1994. The genetic basis of complex human behaviors. Science 264: 1733–1739.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  13. Rushton, J.P., 1995. Race, Evolution and Behavior: A Life-history Perspective. Transaction Publishers, New Brunswick, NJ.Google Scholar
  14. Rushton, J.P. & C.D. Ankney, 1996. Brain size and cognitive ability: correlations with age, sex, social class, and race. Psychometric Bulletin & Review 3: 23–36.Google Scholar
  15. Rushton, J.P. & R.T. Osborne, 1995. Genetic and environmental contributions to cranial capacity estimated in Black and White adolescents. Intelligence 20: 1–13.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Turelli, M., 1984. Heritable genetic variation via mutation-selection balance: Lerch's zeta meets the abdominal bristle. Theoret. Pop. Biol. 25: 138–193.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Wahlsten, D., 1994. The intelligence of heritability. Canadian Psychologist 35: 244–260.Google Scholar
  18. Whitney, G., 1995. Ideology and censorship in behavior genetics. The Mankind Quarterly 35: 327–342. (Twenty-five years of behavior genetics, Presidential Address to Behavior Genetics Association on the occasion of its 25th Annual Meeting: Richmond, VA June 2, 1995.)Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Kluwer Academic Publishers 1997

Authors and Affiliations

  • Robert C. Bailey
    • 1
  1. 1.Department of ZoologyUniversity of Western OntarioLondonCanada

Personalised recommendations