Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology

, Volume 30, Issue 3, pp 257–264 | Cite as

Interval Length and Time-Use by Children with AD/HD: A Comparison of Four Models

Article

Abstract

Predictions made by 4 competing models of time use by children with AD/HD were tested using a computerized version of the Matching Familiar Figures Test in 2 studies. In Study 1 teacher-identified AD/HD children (N = 10) and non-AD/HD controls (N = 10) completed the task under 3 different trial duration conditions (5, 10, and 15 s). In Study 2 the same task was completed by a group of children with a diagnosis of Hyperkinetic Disorder (N = 12) and controls (N = 12). In both studies AD/HD children performed poorly on trials of both 5- and 15-s duration but as well as controls on the 10-s trials. This quadratic function provided support for the State Regulation Deficit model of time use in AD/HD. The value of tailoring the temporal features of learning contexts to the conceptual style of AD/HD children is discussed.

interval length AD/HD task models 

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

REFERENCES

  1. Alberts, E., & Van der Meere, J. J. (1992). Observations of hyperactive behaviour during vigilance. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 33, 1355–1364.Google Scholar
  2. American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: Author.Google Scholar
  3. Antrop, I., Roeyers, H., Van Oost, P., & Buysse, A. (2000). Stimulation seeking and hyperactivity in AD/HD children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 41, 225–232.Google Scholar
  4. Barnett, R., Maruff, P., Vance, A., Luk, E. S. L., Costin, J., Wood, C., et al. (2001). Abnormal executive function in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: The effect of stimulant medication and age on spatial working memory. Psychological Medicine, 31, 1107–1115.Google Scholar
  5. Borger, N., & Van der Meere, J. J. (2000). Visual behaviour of AD/HD children during an attention test: An almost forgotten variable. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 35, 588–596.Google Scholar
  6. Bronfenbrenner, U., & Ceci, S. J. (1994). Nature-nurture reconceptualized in developmental perspective—a bioecological model, Psychological Review, 101, 568–586.Google Scholar
  7. Chee, P., Logan, G., Schachar, R., Lindsey, P., & Wachsmuth, R. (1989). Effects of event rate and display time on sustained attention in hyperactive, normal and control children. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 17, 371–391.Google Scholar
  8. Cirstea, M. C., & Levin, M. F. (2000). Compensatory strategies for reaching in stroke. Brain, 123, 940–953.Google Scholar
  9. Conte, R., Kinsbourne, M., Swanson, J., Zirk, H., & Samuels, M. (1986). Presentation rate effects on pair associate learning by attention deficit disordered children. Child Development, 57, 681–687.Google Scholar
  10. Cornoldi, C., Barbieri, A., Gaiani, C., & Zocchi, S. (1999). Strategic memory deficits in attention deficit disorder with hyperactivity participants: The role of executive processes. Developmental Neuropsychology, 15, 53–71.Google Scholar
  11. Curran, S., Newman, S., Taylor, E., & Asherson, P. (2000). Hypescheme: An operational criteria checklist and minimum data set for molecular genetic studies of attention deficit and hyperactivity disorders. American Journal of Medical Genetics, 96, 244–250.Google Scholar
  12. Dalby, J. T., Kinsbourne, M., & Swanson, J. M. (1989). Self-paced learning in children with attention deficit disorder with hyperactivity. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 17, 269–275.Google Scholar
  13. Dalby, J. T., Kinsbourne, M., Swanson, J., & Sobel, M. P. (1977). Hyperactive children under use of time: Correction by stimulant medication. Child Development, 44, 1448–1453.Google Scholar
  14. Douglas, V.I. (1983). Attentional and cognitive problems. In M. Rutter (Ed.), Developmental Neuro-psychiatry. New York: Guilford.Google Scholar
  15. Douglas, V. I., & Peters, K. G. (1979). Towards a clearer definition of attentional deficit in hyperactive children. In G. A. Hale & M. Lewis (Eds.), Attention and the development of cognitive skills. New York: Plenum.Google Scholar
  16. Elliott, C. D. (1983a). British Ability Scales: Manual 1. Windsor: NFER-Nelson.Google Scholar
  17. Elliott, C. D. (1983b). British Ability Scales: Manual 2. Windsor: NFER-Nelson.Google Scholar
  18. Frank, Y., Seiden, J., & Napolitano, B. (1996). Visual event-related potentials and reaction in normal adults, normal children and children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: Differences in short-term memory processing. International Journal of Neuroscience, 88, 109–124.Google Scholar
  19. Goodman, R. (1997). The strengths and difficulties questionnaire: A research note. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 38, 581–586.Google Scholar
  20. Gottlieb, G. (1991). Experiential canalization of behavioural-development theory. Developmental Psychology, 27, 4–13.Google Scholar
  21. Kagan, J. (1965). Reflection-impulsivity and reading ability in primary grade children. Child Development, 36, 609–628.Google Scholar
  22. Karatekin, C., & Asarnow, R. F. (1998). Working memory in childhood onset schizophrenia and attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Psychiatry Research, 80, 165–176.Google Scholar
  23. Little, T. D. (2000). The millennial challenge: Modelling the agentic self in context. International Journal of Behavioural Development, 24, 149–152.Google Scholar
  24. Nation, K., & Snowling, M. J. (1998). Individual differences in contextual facilitation: Evidence from dyslexia and poor reading comprehension. Child Development, 69, 996–1011.Google Scholar
  25. Nigg, J. (2001). Is ADHD a disinhibitory disorder? Psychological Bulletin, 127, 571–598.Google Scholar
  26. Rubia, K., Taylor, A., Taylor, E., & Sergeant, J. A. (1999). Synchronization, anticipation, and consistency in motor timing of children with dimensionally defined attention deficit hyperactivity behaviour. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 89, 1237–1258.Google Scholar
  27. Rutter, M. (1967). A childrens' behaviour questionnaire for completion by teachers: Preliminary findings. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 8, 1–11.Google Scholar
  28. Schweitzer, J. B., Faber, T. L., Grafton, S. T., Tune, L. E., Hoffman, J. M., & Kilts, C. D. (2000). Alternations in the functional anatomy of working memory in adult attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry, 157, 278–280.Google Scholar
  29. Seidman, L. J., Beiderman, J., Faraone, S. V., Wever, W., & Ouellette, C. (1997). Toward defining a neuropsychology of attention deficit-hyperactivity disorder: Performance of children and adolescents from a large clinically referred sample. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 65, 150–160.Google Scholar
  30. Sergeant, J. (2000). The cognitive-energetic model: An empirical approach to attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 24, 7–12.Google Scholar
  31. Sonuga-Barke, E. J. S. (1994). On dysfunction and function in psychological accounts of childhood disorder. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 35, 801–815.Google Scholar
  32. Sonuga-Barke, E. J. S. (in press). Psychological Heterogeneity in AD/HD: A dual pathway model of behaviour and cognition. Behavioural Brain Research.Google Scholar
  33. Sonuga-Barke, E. J. S., Houlberg, K., & Hall, M. (1994). When is impulsiveness not impulsiveness?: The case of children's cognitive style. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 35, 1247–1253.Google Scholar
  34. Sonuga-Barke, E. J. S., & Taylor, E. (1992). The effect of delay on hyperactive and nonhyperactive childrens response-times. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 33, 1091–1096.Google Scholar
  35. Sonuga-Barke, E. J. S., Williams, E., Hall, M., & Saxton, T. (1996). Hyperactivity and delay aversion III: The effect on cognitive style of imposing delay after errors. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 37, 189–194.Google Scholar
  36. Swanson, J., Sergeant, J., Taylor, E., Sonuga-Barke, E. J. S., Jensen, P., & Cantwell, D. (1998). Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and hyperkinetic disorder. The Lancet, 351, 429–433.Google Scholar
  37. Van der Meere, J. J. (1996). The role of inattention in hyperactivity disorders. In S. Sandberg (Ed.), Monographs on child and adolescent psychiatry: Hyperactivity disorders. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  38. Van der Meere, J., Gunning, B., & Stemerdink, N. (1999). The effect of methylphenidate and clonodine on response inhibition and state regulation in children with AD/HD. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 40, 291–298.Google Scholar
  39. Van der Meere, J., Stemerdink, N., & Gunning, B. (1995). Efects of presentation rate of stimuli on response-inhibition in AD/HD children with and without tics. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 81, 259–262.Google Scholar
  40. Walczyk, J. J. (1995). Testing a compensatory-encoding model. Reading Research Quarterly, 30, 396–408.Google Scholar
  41. World Health Organisation. (1992). Mental and behavioural disorders. Diagnostic criteria for research. In International classification of diseases (10th Ed.). Geneva, Switzerland: Author.Google Scholar
  42. Zec, R. F. (1995). The neuropsychology of aging. Experimental Gerontology, 30, 431–442.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Plenum Publishing Corporation 2002

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Centre for Research into Psychological Development, Department of PsychologyUniversity of Southampton at HighfieldSouthamptonEngland

Personalised recommendations