Journal of Nonverbal Behavior

, Volume 25, Issue 4, pp 261–272 | Cite as

Mustache Fashion Covaries with a Good Marriage Market for Women

  • Nigel Barber

Abstract

It was predicted that men would emphasize sexually-selected traits, including mustaches, beards, and sideburns, when they have difficulty obtaining spouses. Using annual data on British beard fashions extending from 1842–1971, it was found that mustaches, and facial hair in general, are more frequent when there is a good supply of single men of marriageable age. Facial hair fashions, particularly mustaches and beards, were reduced when illegitimacy ratios were high. Regression analyses showed that the relationship between mustache fashion and the marriage market and illegitimacy, respectively, is independent of linear time trend. Results suggest that facial hair is worn to enhance a man's marriage prospects by increasing physical attractiveness and perception of social status. Men shave their mustaches, possibly to convey an impression of trustworthiness, when the marriage market is weak and women might fear sexual exploitation and desertion.

mustache fashion facial hair illegitimacy ratio marital opportunity 

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Addison, W. E. (1989). Beardedness as a factor in perceived masculinity. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 68, 921–922.Google Scholar
  2. Barber, N. (1995). The evolutionary psychology of physical attractiveness: Sexual selection and human morphology. Ethology and Sociobiology, 16. 395–424.Google Scholar
  3. Barber, N. (1998 a). The slender ideal and eating disorders: An interdisciplinary "telescope" model. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 23, 295–307.Google Scholar
  4. Barber, N. (1998 b). Secular changes in standards of bodily attractiveness in women: Tests of a reproductive model. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 23, 449–454 (1998).Google Scholar
  5. Barber, N. (1999 a) Women's dress fashions as a function of reproductive strategy. Sex Roles, 40, 459–471.Google Scholar
  6. Barber, N. (1999 b). Reproductive and occupational stereotypes of bodily curvaceousness and weight. The Journal of Social Psychology, 139, 247–249.Google Scholar
  7. Barber, N. (2000 a). Why parents matter: Parental investment and child outcomes. Westport, CT: Bergin and Garvey.Google Scholar
  8. Barber, N. (2000 b). On the relationship between country sex ratios and teen pregnancy: A Replication. Cross-Cultural Research, 34, 26–37.Google Scholar
  9. Cunningham, M. R., Barbee, A. P., & Pike, C. L. (1990). What do women want? Facialmetric assessment of multiple motives in the perception of male facial physical attractiveness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59, 61–72.Google Scholar
  10. Ekman, P. (1997). Telling lies: Clues to deceit in the marketplace, politics, and marriage. New York: Norton.Google Scholar
  11. Feinman, S., & Gill, G. W. (1977). Female's responses to male beardedness. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 58, 533–534.Google Scholar
  12. Guttentag, M., & Secord, P. F. (1983). Too many women: The sex ratio question. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
  13. Hatfield, E., & Sprecher, S. (1986). Mirror, mirror: The importance of looks in everyday life. Albany, NY: State University of New York Press.Google Scholar
  14. Hellstroem, A., & Tekle, J. (1994). Person perception through facial photographs: Effects of glasses, hair, and beard, on judgments of occupation and personal qualities. European Journal of social Psychology, 24, 693–705.Google Scholar
  15. Jackson, L. A. (1992). Physical appearance and gender. Albany, NY: State University of New York Press.Google Scholar
  16. Kenny, C. T., & Fletcher, D. (1973). Effects of beardedness on person perception. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 37, 413–414.Google Scholar
  17. Klapprott, J. (1976). Barba facit magistrum: an investigation into the effect of a bearded university teacher on his students. Psychologie Schweizerische Zeitschrift fuer Psychologie und ihre Andwendungen, 35, 16–27.Google Scholar
  18. Lowe, E. D., & Lowe, J. W. D. (1990). Velocity of the fashion process in women's formal evening dress, 1789–1980. The Clothing and Textiles Research Journal, 9, 50–58.Google Scholar
  19. Mitchell, B. R. (1988). British historical statistics. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  20. Muscarella, F., & Cunningham, M. R. (1996). The evolutionary significance and social perception of male pattern baldness and facial hair. Ethology ans Sociobiology, 17, 99–117.Google Scholar
  21. Pancer, S. M., & Meindl, J. R. (1978). Length of hair and beardedness as determinants of personality impressions. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 46, 1328–1330.Google Scholar
  22. Pellegrini, R. J. (1973). Impressions of male personality as a function of beardedness. Psychology, 10, 29–33.Google Scholar
  23. Reed, J. A., & Blunk, E. M. (1990). The influence of facial hair on impression formation. Social Behavior and Personality, 18, 169–176.Google Scholar
  24. Robinson, D. E. (1976). Fashions in shaving and trimming of the beard: The men of the Illustrated London News, 1842–1972. American Journal of Sociology, 81, 1133–1141.Google Scholar
  25. Roll, S., & Verinis, J. S. (1971). Stereotypes of scalp and facial hair as measured by the semantic differential. Psychological Reports, 28, 975–980.Google Scholar
  26. Shepperd, J. A., & Strathman, A. J. (1989). Attractiveness and height: the role of stature in dating preference, frequency of dating, and perceptions of attractiveness. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 15, 617–627.Google Scholar
  27. Silverstein, B., Perdue, L., Peterson, B., Vogel, L., and Fantini, D. A. (1986). Possible causes of the thin standard of bodily attractiveness for women. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 5, 907–916.Google Scholar
  28. Stack, S. (1988). The effect of domestic/religious individualism on suicide. In D. L. Thomas (Ed.), The religion and family connection: Social science perspectives pp. 175–204. Provo, UT: Religious Studies Center, Brigham Young University.Google Scholar
  29. Thornhill, R., & Gangestad, S. W. (1994). Human fluctuating asymmetry and sexual behavior. Psychological Science, 5, 297–302.Google Scholar
  30. Wogalter, M. S., & Hosie, J. A. (1991). Effects of cranial and facial hair on perceptions of age and person. The Journal of Social Psychology, 131, 589–591.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Human Sciences Press, Inc. 2001

Authors and Affiliations

  • Nigel Barber

There are no affiliations available

Personalised recommendations