Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology

, Volume 29, Issue 6, pp 541–556

Executive Functioning, Temporal Discounting, and Sense of Time in Adolescents with Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) and Oppositional Defiant Disorder (ODD)

  • Russell A. Barkley
  • Gwenyth Edwards
  • Margaret Laneri
  • Kenneth Fletcher
  • Lori Metevia
Article

Abstract

Clinic-referred teens (ages 12–19) with ADHD and ODD (N = 101) were compared to community control (CC) teens, equated for age and sex, (N = 39) on a variety of psychological tasks assessing executive functioning (EF), temporal reward discounting, and time estimation and reproduction. A factor analysis reduced the EF measures to three dimensions, representing CPT Inattention, Working Memory, and CPT Inhibition. Results indicated that the ADHD group had significantly more CPT Inattention than the CC group. No differences were found for Working Memory or CPT Inhibition. The ADHD group displayed significantly greater temporal discounting of delayed hypothetical monetary rewards relative to immediate ones and manifested more impaired time reproduction, but not time estimation, than did the CC group. Main effects for level of IQ were found only on the Working Memory factor and largely did not interact with the group factor otherwise. The group differences in CPT Inattention, temporal discounting, and time reproduction were not a function of level of comorbid oppositional defiant disorder, delinquency, or anxiety–depression. Results are reasonably consistent with past research on EF and sense of time in children with ADHD and extend these findings to the adolescent age group. Problems with working memory and CPT inhibition found in prior studies of children with ADHD, however, were not evident here, perhaps owing to age-related improvements or insufficient task difficulty.

ADHD attention deficit hyperactivity disorder adolescents executive functioning 

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

REFERENCES

  1. Achenbach, T. M. (1991). Manual for the Child Behavior Checklist/4–18 and 1991 Profile. Burlington, VT: Author.Google Scholar
  2. Ainslie, G. (1992). Picoeconomics: The strategic interaction of successive motivational states within the person. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  3. American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: Author.Google Scholar
  4. Baddeley, A. D. (1986). Working memory. Oxford, UK: Clarendon Press.Google Scholar
  5. Barkley, R. A. (1997). ADHD and the nature of self-control. New York: Guilford.Google Scholar
  6. Barkley, R. A. (1998). Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: A handbook for diagnosis and treatment (2nd ed.). New York: Guilford.Google Scholar
  7. Barkley, R. A. (1999). Response inhibition in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Mental Retardation and Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews, 5, 177–184.Google Scholar
  8. Barkley, R. A. (2000). Genetics of childhood disorders: XVII. ADHD, Part I: The executive functions and ADHD. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 39, 1064–1068.Google Scholar
  9. Barkley, R. A. (2001). The executive functions and self-regulation: An evolutionary neuropsychological perspective. Neuropsychology Review, 11, 1–29.Google Scholar
  10. Barkley, R. A., Anastopoulos, A. D., Guevremont, D. G., & Fletcher, K. F. (1991). Adolescents with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: Patterns of behavioral adjustment, academic functioning, and treatment utilization. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 30, 752–761.Google Scholar
  11. Barkley, R. A., Edwards, G., Laneri, M., Fletcher, K., & Metevia, L. (in press-a). The efficacy of problem-solving training alone, behavior management training alone, and their combination for parent-adolescent conflict in teenagers with ADHD and ODD. Google Scholar
  12. Barkley, R. A., Fischer, M., Edelbrock, C. S., & Smallish, L. (1990). The adolescent outcome of hyperactive children diagnosed by research criteria, I: An 8 year prospective follow-up study. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 29, 546–557.Google Scholar
  13. Barkley, R. A., Koplowitz, S., Anderson, T., & McMurray, M. B. (1997). Sense of time in children with ADHD: Effects of duration, distraction, and stimulant medication. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 3, 359–369.Google Scholar
  14. Barkley, R. A., Murphy, K. R., & Bush, T. (in press-b). Time perception and reproduction in young adults with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Neuropsychology.Google Scholar
  15. Barkley, R. A., Murphy, K. R., & Kwasnik, D. (1996). Psychological adjustment and adaptive impairments in young adults with ADHD. Journal of Attention Disorders, 1, 41–54.Google Scholar
  16. Benton, A. L., & de Hamsher, K. (1978). Multilingual Aphasia Examination. Iowa City: University of Iowa.Google Scholar
  17. Brown, J. W. (1990). Psychology of time awareness. Brain and Cognition, 14, 144–164.Google Scholar
  18. Brown, R. T. (1989). Creativity: What are we to measure? In J. A. Glover, R. R. Ronning, & C. R. Reynolds (Eds.), Handbook of creativity (pp. 3–32). New York: Plenum.Google Scholar
  19. Brown, S. W. (1985). Time perception and attention: The effects of prospective versus retrospective paradigms and task demands on perceived duration. Perception and Psychophysics, 38, 115–124.Google Scholar
  20. Biederman, J., Faraone, S., Milberger, S., Guite, J., Mick, E., Chen, L., Mennin, D., Ouellette, C., Moore, P., Spencer, T., Norman, D., Wilens, T., Kraus, I., & Perrin, J. (1996). A prospective 4–year follow-up study of attention-deficit hyperactivity and related disorders. Archives of General Psychiatry, 53, 437–446.Google Scholar
  21. Cappella, B., Gentile, J. R., & Juliano, D. B. (1977). Time estimation by hyperactive and normal children. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 44, 787–790.Google Scholar
  22. Clark, C., Prior, M., & Kinsella, G. J. (2000). Do executive function deficits differentiate between adolescents with ADHD and oppositional defiant/conduct disorder? A neuropsychological study using the Six Elements Test and Hayling Sentence Completion Test. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 28, 403–414.Google Scholar
  23. Conners, C. K. (1993). The Conners ContinuousPerformance Test. North Tonawanda, NY: Multi-Health Systems.Google Scholar
  24. Corkum, P. V., & Siegel, L. S. (1993). Is the continuous performance task a valuable research tool for use with children with attention-deficithyperactivity disorder? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 34, 1217–1239.Google Scholar
  25. Dooling-Litfin, J. (1997). Time perception in children with ADHD. The ADHD Report, 5(5), 13–16.Google Scholar
  26. Edwards, G., Barkley, R. A., Laneri, M., Fletcher, K., & Metevia, L. (2001). Parent-adolescent conflicts in teenagers with ADHD and ODD: The role of parental maladjustment. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 26, 557–572.Google Scholar
  27. Fischer, M., Barkley, R. A., Edelbrock, C. S., & Smallish, L. (1990). The adolescent outcome of hyperactive children diagnosed by research criteria, II: Academic, attentional, and neuropsychological status. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 58, 580–588.Google Scholar
  28. Fraisse, P. (1963). The psychology of time. New York: Harper & Row.Google Scholar
  29. Fuster, J. M. (1997). The prefrontal cortex. New York: Raven Press.Google Scholar
  30. Gerbing, D. W., Ahadi, S. A., & Patton, J. H. (1987). Toward a conceptualization of impulsivity: Components across the behavioral and self-report domains. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 22, 357–379.Google Scholar
  31. Gittelman, R., Mannuzza, S., Shenker, R., & Bonagura, N. (1985). Hyperactive boys almost grown up: I. Psychiatric status. Archives of General Psychiatry, 42, 937–947.Google Scholar
  32. Green, L., Fry, A. F., & Myerson, J. (1994). Discounting of delayed rewards: A life-span comparison. Psychological Science, 5, 33–36.Google Scholar
  33. Green, L., Myerson, J., Lichtman, D., Rosen, S., & Fry, A. (1996). Temporal discounting in choice between delayed rewards: The role of age and income. Psychology and Aging, 11, 79–84.Google Scholar
  34. Grodzinsky, G. M., & Diamond, R. (1992). Frontal lobe functioning in boys with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Developmental Neuropsychology, 8, 427–445.Google Scholar
  35. Kaufman, A., & Kaufman, N. (1993). Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.Google Scholar
  36. Klorman, R., Hazel-Fernandez, L. A., Shaywitz, S. E., Fletcher, J. M., Marchione, K. E., Holahan, J. M., Stuebing, K. K., & Shaywitz, B. A. (1999). Executive functioning deficits in attention-deficit/ hyperactivity disorder are independent of oppositional defiant and reading disorder. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 38, 1148–1155.Google Scholar
  37. Levine, M., & Spivack, G. (1959). Incentive, time conception and selfcontrol in a group of emotionall disturbed boys. Journal of clinical Psychology, 8, 110–113.Google Scholar
  38. Levine, M., Spivack, G., Fuschillo, J., & Tavernier, A. (1959). Intelligence and measures of inhibition and time sense. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 7, 224–226.Google Scholar
  39. Lezak, M. (1995). Neuropsychological assessment (3rd ed.). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  40. Logue, A. W. (1988). Research on self-control: An integrating framework. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 11, 665–679.Google Scholar
  41. Losier, B. J., McGrath, P. J., & Klein, R. M. (1996). Error patterns on the continuous performance test in nonmedicated and medicated samples of children with and without ADHD: A meta-analytic review. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 37, 971–987.Google Scholar
  42. Lyon, G. R., & Krasnegor, N. (1996). Attention, memory, and executive function. Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes.Google Scholar
  43. MacLeod, D., & Prior, M. (1996). Attention deficits in adolescents with ADHD and other clinical groups. Child Neuropsychology, 2, 1–10.Google Scholar
  44. Mariani, M., & Barkley, R. A. (1997). Neuropsychological and academic functioning in preschool children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Developmental Neuropsychology, 13, 111–129.Google Scholar
  45. Mazur, J. E. (1987). An adjusting procedure for studying delayed reinforcement. In M. L. Commons, J. E. Mazur, J. A. Nevin, & H. Rachlin (Eds.), Quantitative analyses of behavior: Vol. 5, the effect of delay and of intervening events on reinforcement value (pp. 55–73). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  46. Mimura, M., Kinsbourne, M., & O'Connor,M. (2000). Time estimation by patients with frontal lesions and by Korskoff amnesics. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 6, 517–528.Google Scholar
  47. Mischel, W., Shoda, Y., & Rodriguez, M. (1989). Delay of gratification in children. Science, 244, 933–938.Google Scholar
  48. Montare, A. (1977). Human temporal behavior and discriminationreversal learning. Pavlovian Journal of Biological Science, 12, 232–246.Google Scholar
  49. Murphy, K. R., Barkley, R. A., & Bush, T. (2001). Executive functioning and olfactory identification in young adults with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Neuropsychology, 15, 211–220.Google Scholar
  50. Nigg, J. T. (1999). The ADHD response-inhibition deficit as measured by the stop task: Replication with DSM-IV Combined Type, extension, and qualification. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 27, 393–402.Google Scholar
  51. Nigg, J. T., Hinshaw, S. P., Carte, E. T., & Treuting, J. J. (1998). Neuropsychological correlates of childhood attention-deficit/ hyperactivity disorder: Explainable by comorbid disruptive behavior or reading problems? Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 107, 468–480.Google Scholar
  52. Pavlov, I. P. (1927). Conditioned reflexes. London: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  53. Rachlin, H., & Ranieri, A. (1992). Irrationality, impulsiveness, and selfishness as discount reversal effects. In G. Loewenstein & J. Elster (Eds.), Choice over time (pp. 93–118). New York: Sage.Google Scholar
  54. Rapport, M. D., & Kelly, K. L. (1993). Psychostimulant effects on learning and cognitive performance. In J. L. Matson (Ed.), Handbook of hyperactivity in children (pp. 97–136). Boston, MA: Allyn & Bacon.Google Scholar
  55. Roberts, R. J., & Pennington, B. F. (1996). An integrative framework for examining prefrontal cognitive processes. Developmental Neuropsychology, 12, 105–126.Google Scholar
  56. Seidman, L. J., Biederman, J., Faraone, S. V., Milberger, S., Seiverd, K., Benedict, K., Bernstein, J. H., Weber, W., & Ouellette, C. (1996). Toward defining a neuropsychology of ADHD: Performance of children and adolescents from a large clinically referred sample. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 65, 150–160.Google Scholar
  57. Seigman, A. W. (1961). The relationship between future perspective, time estimation, and impulse control in a group of young offenders and in a control group. Journal of Consulting Psychology, 25, 470–475.Google Scholar
  58. Senior, N., Towne, D., & Huessy, D. (1979). Time estimation and hyperactivity, a replication. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 49, 289–290.Google Scholar
  59. Shaw, G., & Brown, G. (1999). Arousal, time estimation, and time use in attention-disordered children. Developmental Neuropsychology, 16, 227–242.Google Scholar
  60. Walker, N. W. (1982). Comparison of cognitive tempo and time estimation by young boys. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 54, 715–722.Google Scholar
  61. Wechlser, D. (1994). The Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-III (3rd ed.). Odessa, TX: Psychological Corp.Google Scholar
  62. Weiss, G., & Hechtman, L. T. (1993). Hyperactive children grown up (2nd ed.). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
  63. Zakay, D. (1990). The evasive art of subjective time measurement: Some methodological dilemmas. In R. A. Block (Ed.), Cognitive models of psychological time (pp. 59–84). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  64. Zakay, D. (1992). The role of attention in children's time perception. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 54, 355–371.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Plenum Publishing Corporation 2001

Authors and Affiliations

  • Russell A. Barkley
    • 1
  • Gwenyth Edwards
    • 1
  • Margaret Laneri
    • 1
  • Kenneth Fletcher
    • 1
  • Lori Metevia
    • 1
  1. 1.Department of PsychiatryUniversity of Massachusetts Medical SchoolWorcester

Personalised recommendations