Minds and Machines

, Volume 11, Issue 2, pp 155–195

The Epigenesis of Meaning in Human Beings, and Possibly in Robots

  • Jordan Zlatev
Article

Abstract

This article addresses a classical question: Can a machine use language meaningfully and if so, how can this be achieved? The first part of the paper is mainly philosophical. Since meaning implies intentionality on the part of the language user, artificial systems which obviously lack intentionality will be `meaningless' (pace e.g. Dennett). There is, however, no good reason to assume that intentionality is an exclusively biological property (pace e.g. Searle) and thus a robot with bodily structures, interaction patterns and development similar to those of human beings would constitute a system possibly capable of meaning – a conjecture supported through a Wittgenstein-inspired thought experiment. The second part of the paper focuses on the empirical and constructive questions. Departing from the principle of epigenesis stating that during every state of development new structure arises on the basis of existing structure plus various sorts of interaction, a model of human cognitive and linguistic development is proposed according to which physical, social and linguistic interactions between the individual and the environment have their respective peaks in three consecutive stages of development: episodic, mimetic and symbolic. The transitions between these stages are qualitative, and bear a similarity to the stages in phylogenesis proposed by Donald (1991) and Deacon (1997). Following the principle of epigenetic development, robotogenesis could possibly recapitulate ontogenesis, leading to the emergence of intentionality, consciousness and meaning.

consciousness development embodiment epigenesis intentionality language meaning robotics social interaction the Turing Test 

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Baldwin, J. (1913), Social and Ethical Interpretations in Mental Development. A Study in Social Psychology, 5th ed. New York: MacMillan Press.Google Scholar
  2. Balkenius, C. (1995), Natural Intelligence in Artificial Creatures, Lund University Cognitive Studies 37.Google Scholar
  3. Balkenius, C. (forthcoming), ‘Attention, Habituation and Conditioning: Towards a Computational Model’, Cognitive Science Quarterly 1.Google Scholar
  4. Bates, E. (1979), The Emergence of Symbols. Cognition and Communication in Infancy. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  5. Bates, B. (in press), ‘On the Nature and Nurture of Language’, in E. Bizzi, P. Calissano, and V. Volterra, eds., Frontiere della Biologia The Brain of Homo sapiens, Rome: Giovanni Trecani.Google Scholar
  6. Bateson, M. (1975), ‘Mother-infant Exchanges: The Epigenesis of Conversational interaction’ in D. Aaronson and R. Rieber, eds., Developmental Psycholinguistics and Communication Disorders. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences (Vol. 263). New York: New York Academy of Sciences.Google Scholar
  7. Bloom, P. (1993), ‘Language Development’, Handbook of Psycholinguistics, New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  8. Brooks, R., Breazeal, C., Marjanovic, M, Scassellati, B. and Williamson, M. (1999), ‘The Cog Project: Building a Humanoid Robot’, in C.L. Nehaniv, ed., Computation for Metaphors, Analogy, and Agents, Springer-Verlag Lecture Notes in Computer Science, 1562.Google Scholar
  9. Bruner, J. (1983), Child's Talk, New York: Norton.Google Scholar
  10. Bruner, J. (1986), Actual Minds, Possible Worlds, Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  11. Buford, T. (1971), Essays on Other Minds, University of Illinois Press.Google Scholar
  12. Butterworth, G. (1991), ‘The Ontogeny and Philogeny of Joint Visual Attention’, in A. Whiten, ed., Natural Theories of Mind. London: Basil Blackwell.Google Scholar
  13. Butterworth, G. (1995), ‘An Ecological Perspective on the Origins of the Self’ in J.L. Bermudez, A. Marcel, and N. Eilan, eds., The Body and the Self, Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  14. Carruthers, P. (1996), Language, Thought and Consciousness, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  15. Carruthers, P. (1998), Mind and Language 13(4), 457–476.Google Scholar
  16. Changeux, J. (1985), Neuronal Man, New York: Pantheon Books.Google Scholar
  17. Chomsky, N. (1980), Rules and Representations, New York: Columbia University Press.Google Scholar
  18. Churchland, P.M. and Churchland, P.S. (1990), ‘Could a Machine Think?’ Scientific American, January 1990: 26–31.Google Scholar
  19. Clark, H.H. and Marshall, C.R. (1981) ‘Definite Reference and Mutual Knowledge’, in Joshi, Webber and Sag, eds., Elements of Discourse Understanding, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  20. Curtiss, S. (1977), Genie: A Psycholinguistic Study of a Modern-day “Wild Child”. London: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  21. Deacon, T. (1997), The Symbolic Species: The Co-evolution of Language and the Brain, New York: Norton.Google Scholar
  22. Dennett, D. (1987), The Intentional Stance, Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  23. Dennett, D. (1991), Consciousness Explained, Boston: Little, Brown.Google Scholar
  24. Dennett, D. (1995), ‘Cog: Steps Towards Consciousness in Robots’ in T. Metzinger, ed., Conscious Erperience. Lawrence, Kansas: Allen Press.Google Scholar
  25. Dennett, D. (1996), Kinds of Minds. Towards an Understanding of Consciousness, London: Phenix.Google Scholar
  26. Donald, M. (1991), Origins of the Modern Mind. Three Stages in the Evolution of Culture and Cognition, Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  27. Drescher, G. (1991), Made-up Minds. A Constructivist Approach to Artificial Intelligence, Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  28. Dreyfus, H. (1991), Being-in-the-world. A Commentary on Heidegger's “Being and Time, Division I”, Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  29. Dreyfus, H. (1993) [1972], What Computers (Still) Can't Do. A Critique of Artificial Reason, Third revised edition. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  30. Dunbar, R. (1996), Grooming, Gossip and the Evolution of Language, London: Faber and Faber.Google Scholar
  31. Edelman, G. (1989), The Remembered Present. A Biological Theory of Consciousness. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
  32. Edelman, G. (1992), Bright Air, Brilliant Fire. On the Matter of the Mind, New York: Basic Books, HarperCollins Publications.Google Scholar
  33. Fenson, L., Dale, P.S., Reznick, J.S., Bates, E., Thal, D. and Pethick, S.J. (1994), ‘Variability in Early Communicative Development’. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Developjnent, Serial No. 242. Vol. 59, No. 5.Google Scholar
  34. Firth, U. (1989), ‘A New Look at Language and Communication in Autism’, British Journal of Disorders of Communication 24, 123–150.Google Scholar
  35. Fodor, J.A. (1983), The Modularity of Mind, Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  36. Gallup, G. (1970), ‘Chimpanzees: Self recognition’, Science 167, 86–87.Google Scholar
  37. Gibson, J.J. (1979), The Ecological Approach to Visual Perception, Boston: Houghton Mifflin.Google Scholar
  38. Gómez, J. (1994), ‘Mutual Awareness in Primate Communication: A Gricean approach’, in S. Parker, R. Mitchel and M. Boccia, eds., Self-awareness in Animals and Humans. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  39. Gopnik. A., Choi, S. and Baumberger, T. (1996), ‘Cross-linguistic Differences in Early Semantic and Cognitive Development’. Cognitive Development 11(2), 197–227.Google Scholar
  40. Grice, P. (1957), ‘Meaning’, Philosophical Review 66, 377–388.Google Scholar
  41. Gärdenfors, P. (2000), Conceptual Spaces. The Geometry of Thought, Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  42. Haeckel, E. (1874), The Evolution of Man: A Popular Exposition of the Principle Points of Human Ontogeny and Phylogeny, New York: International Science Library.Google Scholar
  43. Harnad, S. (1991), ‘Other Bodies, Other Minds: A Machine Incarnation of an Old Philosophical Problem’, Minds and Machines 1, 43–54.Google Scholar
  44. Harnad, S. (1993). ‘Grounding Symbols in the Analog World with Neural Nets’, Think 2(1), 12–78.Google Scholar
  45. Harris, P. (1991), ‘The Work of Imagination’ in A. Whiten, ed., Natural Theories of Mind. London: Basil Blackwell.Google Scholar
  46. Hayes, C. (1951), The Ape in Our House, New York: Harper.Google Scholar
  47. Hebb, D.O. (1955), ‘Drive and the C.N.S.’ Psychological Review 62, 243–254.Google Scholar
  48. Ingram, D. (1989), First Language Acquisition. Method, Description, and Explanation. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  49. Itkonen, E. (1983), Causality in Linguistic Theory. A Critical Investigation into the Philosophical and Methodological Foundations of ‘Non-autonomous’ Linguistics. Bloomington: Indiana University Press.Google Scholar
  50. Itkonen, E. (1997), ‘The Social Ontology of Meaning’, SKY 1997 Yearbook of the Linguistic Association of Finland, 49–80.Google Scholar
  51. Jusezyk, P.W. (1997), The Discovery of Spoken Language, Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  52. Kozima, H. (1999), ‘Attention-Sharing, Behavior-Sharing and the Acquisition of Language’. Paper presented at the international symposium on “The ecology of language acquisition”, January 1999, Amsterdam.Google Scholar
  53. Leder, D. (1990), The Absent Body. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
  54. Lenat, D. and Feigenbaum, E. (1991), ‘On the Thresholds of Knowledge’, Artificial Intelligence 47(1–3).Google Scholar
  55. Leneberg, E. (1980), ‘Of Language Knowledge, Apes and Brains’ in T. Sebeok and J. Sebeok, eds., ‘Speaking of Apes’. New York: Plenum Press.Google Scholar
  56. Lewis, M. and J. Brooks-Gunn (1979), Social Cognition and the Acquisition of the Self. New York: Plenum. Liège, August 11–15, 1997.Google Scholar
  57. Locke, J. (1993), ‘Phases in the Child's Development of Language’, American Scientist 82, 436–445.Google Scholar
  58. Malcolm, N. (1971), ‘Knowledge of Other Minds’, in T. Buford, ed., Essays on Other Minds. University of Illinois Press.Google Scholar
  59. Mead, G. (1934), Mind, Self and Society. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
  60. Meltzoff, A. and Moore, M. (1977), ‘Imitation of Facial and Manual Gestures by Human Neonates’, Science 198, 75–78.Google Scholar
  61. Meltzoff, A. and Moore, M. (1995), ‘Infants' Understanding of People and Things: From Body Imitation to Folk Psychology’, in J.L. Bermudez, A. Marcel and N. Eilan, eds., The Body and the Self. Cambridge, Mass.: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
  62. Müller, R. (1996), ‘Innateness, Autonomy, Universality? Neurological Approaches to Language’, Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19, 611–675.Google Scholar
  63. Neisser, U. (1988), ‘Five Kinds of Self-knowledge’, Philosophical Psychology 1, 35–59.Google Scholar
  64. Neisser, U. (1995). ‘The Self Perceived’, in U. Neisser, ed., The Perceived Self. Ecological and lnterpersonal Sources of Self knowledge. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  65. Piaget, J. (1953), The Origin of Intelligence in the Child. London: Routledge and Kegan Paul.Google Scholar
  66. Pierce, C.S. (1931–35), The Collected Papers of Charles Sanders Peirce. Vols. 1–4. Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  67. Pinker, S. (1994), The Language Instinct. New York: William Morrow.Google Scholar
  68. Premack, D. (1976), Intelligence in Ape and Man. Hillsdale, NJ: Laurence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  69. Putnam, H. (1960), Minds and Machines, in S. Hook, ed., Dimensions of Mind. New York: New York University Press.Google Scholar
  70. Rapaport, W. (1988), ‘Syntactic Semantics: Foundations of computational natural language understanding’, in H. Fetzer, ed., Aspects of Artificial lntelligence. Dordrecht: Kluwer.Google Scholar
  71. Regier, T. (1996), The Human Semantic Potential: Spatial Language and Constrained Connectionism. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  72. Reid, T. (1912) [1785], ‘Essays on the Intellectual Powers of Man’, in B. Rand, ed., The Classical Psychologists. Cambridge: Riverside Press.Google Scholar
  73. Rizzolatti, G. and Arbib, M. (1998), ‘Language within our Grasp’, Trends in Neurosciences 21, 188–194.Google Scholar
  74. Ryle, G. (1949), The Concept of Mind. London: Hutchinson's University Library.Google Scholar
  75. Saussure, F. de (1916), Cours de Linguistigue Générale. Paris: Payot.Google Scholar
  76. Savage-Rumbaugh, S., Shanker, S. and Taylor, T. (1998), Apes, Language and the Human Mind. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  77. Scassellati, B. (in press), ‘Investigating Models of Social Development Using a Humanoid Robot’, Presented at the 1998 AAAI Fall Symposium “Robots and Biology: Developing Connections” in Orlando, Florida.Google Scholar
  78. Schoenemann, P. (1999), ‘Syntax as an Emergent Characteristic of the Evolution of Semantic Complexity’, Minds and Machines 9(3), 309–346.Google Scholar
  79. Searle, J. (1980), ‘Minds, Brains and Programs’. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3, 417–24.Google Scholar
  80. Searle, J. (1995), ‘The Mystery of Consciousness’, The New York Review of Books, November 2, 1995: 61–66.Google Scholar
  81. Tomasello, M. (1995), ‘On the Interpersonal Origins of the Self-concept’, in U. Neisser, ed., The Perceived Self. Ecological and interpersonal Sources of Self-knowledge. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  82. Tomasello, M. (1999), The Cultural Origins of Human Cognition, Cambridge, MA: Harvard University PressGoogle Scholar
  83. Tomasello, M., Kdruger, A. and Ratner, H. (1993), ‘Cultural Learning’, Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16(3), 495–511.Google Scholar
  84. Tulving, E. (1985), ‘How Many Memory Systems are There?’, American Psychologist, April 1985: 385–398.Google Scholar
  85. Turing, A. (1950), ‘Computing Machinery and Intelliegnce’, Mind 59, 433–460.Google Scholar
  86. von Glasersfeld, E. (1997), ‘Anticipation and the Constructivist Theory of Cognition’, Presented at International Conference on Computing Anticinatory Systems.Google Scholar
  87. von Hofsten, C. (1989), ‘Transition Mechanisms in Sensorimotor Development’, in A. de Ribaupierre, ed., Transition Mechanisms in Child Development. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  88. Vygotsky, L. (1978), Mind in Society. The Development of Higher Psychological Processes. Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  89. Vygotsky, L. (1986) [1934], Thought and Language. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  90. Wittgenstein, L. (1953), Philosophical Investigations. Oxford: Basil Blackwell.Google Scholar
  91. Wittgenstein, L. (1969), The Blue and Brown Books. London: Basil Blackwell.Google Scholar
  92. Zlatev, J. (1997), Situated Embodiment. Studies in the Emergence of Spatial Meaning. Ph.D. Thesis. Stockholm University, Stockholm: Gotab.Google Scholar
  93. Zlatev, J. (2000), ‘The Mimetic Origins of Self-Conciousness in phylo-, onto-and robotogenesis’, Paper presented at Third Asia-Pacific Conference on Simulated Evolution and Learning (SEAL2000), Nagoya, Japan.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Kluwer Academic Publishers 2001

Authors and Affiliations

  • Jordan Zlatev
    • 1
    • 2
  1. 1.Department of LinguisticsLund UniversitySweden
  2. 2.Department of LinguisticsChulalongkorn UniversityThailand

Personalised recommendations