Journal of Psychology and Judaism

, Volume 24, Issue 3, pp 233–243 | Cite as

Is There a Cognitive Basis for Religious Belief?

  • Ronald L. Goldman
Article

Abstract

This paper explores whether the psychological study of cognition can provide insight into how religious beliefs are formed and maintained. A review of illustrative findings from the research literature suggests three themes: First, regularities in our everyday experience are primarily identified through assessment of individual situations rather than through use of all relevant data. Second, preconceptions are highly important in shaping perceptions and interpretations of new experience. Finally, intense emotions often generate an inaccurate sense of certainty regarding beliefs congruent with these emotions. The applicability of these themes to the relationship between religious experience and religious belief is examined. The paper concludes by briefly discussing whether the increasing proportion of the population receiving statistical and scientific training will influence the level of religious belief in Western culture.

religion religious belief assessment of covariation emotion judgment 

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

REFERENCES

  1. Alloy, L. B., & Tabachnik, N. (1984). Assessment of covariation by humans and animals: The joint influence of prior expectations and current situational information. Psychological Review, 91, 112–149.Google Scholar
  2. Baddeley, A. (1997). Human memory: Theory and practice. East Sussex: Psychology Press. Ch. 10.Google Scholar
  3. Camerer, C. F., & Johnson, E. J. (1991). The process-performance paradox in expert judgment: How can experts know so much and predict so badly? In K. A. Ericsson & J. Smith (Eds.), Toward a general theory of expertise: Prospects and limits (pp. 195–217). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  4. Chapman, L. J., & Chapman, J. P., (1967). Genesis of popular but erroneous diagnostic observations. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 72, 193–204.Google Scholar
  5. Chapman, L. J., & Chapman, J. P. (1969). Illusory correlation as an obstacle to the use of valid psychodiagnostic signs. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 74, 271–280.Google Scholar
  6. Crocker, J. (1981). Judgment of covariation by social perceivers. Psychological Bulletin, 90, 272–292.Google Scholar
  7. Damasio, A. R. (1994). Descartes' error: emotion, reason, and the human brain. New York: Avon Press.Google Scholar
  8. Dawes, R. M., Faust, D., & Meehl, P. E. (1989). Clinical versus actuarial judgment. Science, 243, 1668–1674.Google Scholar
  9. Evan J. S., Over, D. E., & Manktelow, K. I. (1993). Reasoning, decision making and rationality. Cognition, 49, 165–187.Google Scholar
  10. Fischhoff, B., Slovic, P., & Lichtenstein, S. (1977). Knowing with certainty. The appropriateness of extreme confidence. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 3, 552–564.Google Scholar
  11. Freedman, E. G., & Smith L., D. (1996). The role of data and theory in covariation assessment: implications for the theory-ladenness of observation. Journal of Mind and Behavior, 17; 321–343.Google Scholar
  12. Gellner, E. (1985). The psychoanalytic movement. London: Grafton.Google Scholar
  13. Hare, R. D., & Quinn, M. J. (1971). Psychopathy and autonomic conditioning. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 77, 223–235.Google Scholar
  14. Holsopple, J. G., & Phelan, J. G. (1954). The skills of clinicians in analysis of projective tests. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 10, 307–320.Google Scholar
  15. Holyoak, K. J., & Nisbett, R. E. (1988). Induction. In R. J. Sternberg & E. E. Smith (Eds.), The psychology of human thought. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  16. Hume, D. (1988). An enquiry concerning human understanding. Amherst, NY: Prometheus Books. Section X.Google Scholar
  17. James, W. (1958). The varieties of religious experience. New York: New American Library.Google Scholar
  18. Jennings, S., Amabile, T. M., & Ross, L. (1982). Informal covariation assessment: Data based vs. theorybased judgments. In D. Kahneman, P. Slovic, & A. Tversky (Eds.), Judgment under uncertainty: Heuristics and biases. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  19. Johnson-Abercrombie, M. L. (1960). The anatomy of judgment. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
  20. Johnson-Laird, P. N. & Oatley, K. (1992). Basic emotions, rationality, and folk theory. Cognition and Emotion, 6, 210–223.Google Scholar
  21. Kahnemann, D., & Tversky, A. (1973). On the psychology of prediction. Psychological Review, 80, 237–251.Google Scholar
  22. Marr, D. (1982). Vision: A computational investigation into the human representation and processing of visual information: San Francisco: W.H. Freeman.Google Scholar
  23. Meehl, P. E. (1954). Clinical versus statistical prediction: A theoretical analysis and a review of the evidence. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press.Google Scholar
  24. Meehl, P. E. (1986). Causes and effects of my disturbing little book. Journal of Personality Assessment, 50, 370–375.Google Scholar
  25. Nisbett, R. E. et al. (1987). Teaching Reasoning. Science, 238, 625–631.Google Scholar
  26. Nisbett, R. E., & Ross, L. (1980). Human inference: Strategies and shortcomings of social judgment. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.Google Scholar
  27. Oskamp, S. (1965). Overconfidence in case-study judgments. Journal of Consulting Psychology, 29, 261–265.Google Scholar
  28. Rogers, C. R. (1961). On becoming a person. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.Google Scholar
  29. Shaklee, H. & Mims, S. (1982). Sources of error in judging event covariation: effects of memory demands. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 8:208–224.Google Scholar
  30. Shaver, P., Lenauer, M., & Sadd, S. (1980). Religiousness, conversion, and subjective well-being: The “healthy-minded” religion of modern American women. American Journal of Psychiatry, 137, 1563–1568.Google Scholar
  31. Simon, H. A. (1983). Alternate visions of rationality. In H.A. Simon, Reason in human affairs. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
  32. Simon, H. A. (1979). Models of thought. New Haven: Yale University Press.Google Scholar
  33. Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1971). The belief in the “law of small numbers.” Psychological Bulletin, 76, 105–110.Google Scholar
  34. Ward, W. C., & Jenkins, H. M. (1965). The display of information and the judgment of contingency. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 19, 231–241.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Human Sciences Press, Inc. 2000

Authors and Affiliations

  • Ronald L. Goldman

There are no affiliations available

Personalised recommendations